self-control – Education & Teacher Conferences Skip to main content
“Good Ideas” that Might Waste Time
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

I would LOVE to write the following blog posts:

  • Playing with Puppies Improves Learning!
  • A Glass of Expensive Red Wine Each Day Makes You a Better Teacher
  • Research shows: Reading a Good Book on your Couch is the Best Way to Plan Lessons

In other words: if I can say that “research supports a REALLY popular idea,” I make everyone happy.

Alas, my job is often to say the opposite. As in:

Here’s a really popular idea — an idea that sounds like common sense. And: this popular idea probably is not true.

So:

Today I want to explore another “probably not true” idea — one that’s important, and a little bit tricky to understand. Here goes…

Self-Control, and Self-Control

Psychology researchers spend lots of time thinking about “self-control.” Sadly, this compound word has at least two different meanings.

  • Meaning #1: Let’s say you’re the kind of person who — naturally, without really thinking about it, most of the time — resists the tempting thing and does what you ought to do.

We could say that “self-control” is a consistent personality trait you have.

  • Meaning #2: Perhaps you’re standing in front of a plate of donuts. You say to yourself: “I’m SO HUNGRY, and the chocolate-y deliciousness looks amazing. BUT, I’m trying to get in shape, so I’ll take the fruit cup instead…”

We could say that you’re exhibiting “self-control” as a short-term behavior; you’re in a sort of state of self-control.

To distinguish between these two definitions, scholars write about

  • Personality TRAIT self-control, and
  • Short-term STATE self-control.

Although those two descriptors rhyme, they describe different kinds of self-control. One (“trait”) is a stable, ongoing, often unthinking self-control. The other (“state”) is short-term, contingent, self aware self-control.

We have at least three reasons to care about trait vs. state self-control.

  1. TRAIT self-control correlates with all sorts of good outcomes. People with high trait self-control live longer, get better grades, and earn more money. They floss more regularly and probably are nicer to dogs. (I think I made up that last one, but it could be true.)
  2. It just seems obvious: TRAIT self-control is (probably) just lots-n-lots of STATE self-control moments all strung together. As Michael Inzlicht and Brent W. Roberts write:
    • Conventional wisdom has it that people high in trait self-control reap all these benefits because they engage in more state self-control.
  3. For this reason, we have an obvious, common sense path to follow. If we train students to develop moment-by-moment STATE self-control, the result will be stable TRAIT self-control. “Grit,” anyone? All that gritty training means that students will get the benefits listed above: grades and jobs and cavity-free teeth and the love of dogs.

But WAIT JUST A MINUTE…

What if that popular, commonsense hypothesis just isn’t true?

How Can That Be?

No, really: how could that commonsense hypothesis not be true? It’s just OBVIOUS that trait self-control results from moment-by-moment states of self-control. I mean: isn’t it?

In the brief (highly readable) article linked above, Inzlicht and Roberts give us plenty of reasons to doubt that conclusion.

  1. People who score high on tests of trait self-control don’t spend much of their day exercising self-control. (See, for instance, this study.)
  2. Over longer periods of time (months and years), training in state self-control doesn’t result in all those predicted positive outcomes. People go back to their initial patterns.
    • That is: I might be able to avoid donuts long enough to fit into my wedding suit — but in the months after my wedding, I’m likely to put those pounds back on again.

Inzlicht and Roberts share our puzzlement at this odd set of outcomes. (Their article literally includes the subheading: “What the hell is going on here?”)

They speculate, for instance, that the wording has got us confused. Just because we use the compound word “self-control” for both Trait X and State X, our labels don’t necessarily mean that the same mental process is happening below the label.

They propose this thought experiment: imagine that we had decided to use the word “planfulness” instead of “trait self-control.” Because the words are different, we might not end up so surprised that “planfulness” doesn’t result from “state self-control.”

Whatever the reason for our mistake, we should be prepared to acknowledge that our commonsense belief — “training students in state self-control enhances their trait self-control” — probably isn’t true.

Why The Distinction Matters

If we were confident that state-self-control training leads to trait self-control, then we should (almost certainly) take the time to enact that training. So Many Benefits!

But — at least so far — we shouldn’t be confident that frequent bouts of short-term (state) self-control ultimately train up long-term (trait) self-control.

If we mostly want to create trait self-control…well…we just don’t yet know how to do that. And we shouldn’t fool ourselves that that state self-control gets the job done.

(Research, in fact, offers this substantial benefit: it often gives us helpful correctives precisely to stop us from fooling ourselves.)

Now, we should also admit that we want students to practice short-term state self-control because that too is useful. State self-control:

  • gets homework done (when students want to be doing something else), and
  • helps students focus in class (ditto), and
  • keeps students off Tik Tok (when they really want to be on Tik Tok).

And so forth. So I don’t think we give up on self-control all together.

But when we hear someone claim that “grit” can be trained…I think we should ask for good evidence that this claim is true.


Inzlicht, M., & Roberts, B. W. (2024). The fable of state self-control. Current Opinion in Psychology, 101848.

Marshmallows and Beyond: Cultural Influences on Self-Regulation
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Few psychology studies have created a bigger stir than Walter Mishel’s research into marshmallows.

Okay, he was really doing research into self-control.

But the marshmallow images were adorable: all those cute children desperately trying not to eat one marshmallow right now, so that they’d get two marshmallows in fifteen minutes.

Mishel’s studies got so much attention because they suggested that self-control correlates with SO MANY good things: high grades, better jobs, better health, etc.

And, they suggested that self-control is relatively stable. Some studies suggested that the marshmallow test, given at to a child at age five, could offer insights into their lives decades later.

Now, this research pool includes lots of complexity.

If, for instance, you saw Dr. Mishel at our 2015 conference in Boston, you know that trustworthiness matters.

Children waited for the 2nd marshmallow more often if they had reason to believe that the experimenter would actually follow through on their commitments. (Smart kids!)

So, do other factors matter?

The Power of Culture

A research team in Japan, led by Kaichi Yanaoka, wondered if cultural factors might shape self control.

So, for instance, in Japan waiting for food gets cultural priority — much more so than in the United States (where Mishel did his research).

But, Japanese culture does not emphasize waiting to open gifts as much as families in the US often do.

For instance, as Yanaoka explains in this study, Japanese parents often leave gifts for their children, with no cultural expectation that the children should wait to open them.

So, do these cultural differences shape performance on the marshmallow test?

Hypothesis. Data.

Based on these cultural norms, team Yanaoka hypothesized that children from the US would be better at waiting to open gifts, but worse at waiting to eat marshmallows, than their Japanese counterparts.

Because research requires precision, this study includes LOTS of details. (For instance, the researchers checked to be sure that the Japanese children had eaten marshmallows before, so they knew what temptation they were resisting.)

But the overall design was quite simple. In the US and Japan, children waited either to eat marshmallows, or to open gifts. Researchers followed a simple script:

Now it’s gift time! You have a choice for your gift today. You can either have this one gift to open right now, or if you wait for me to get more gifts from the other room, you can have two gifts to open instead. […]

Stay right there in that chair and I’ll leave this right here, and if you haven’t opened it […] before I get back, you can two to open instead.

Of course, for the children getting marshmallows, the script said “marshmallow” and “eat” rather than “gift” and “open.”

So, what did the researchers find?

Sure enough, cultural expectations shape self control.

In this case, Japanese children waited for the second marshmallow (median time: 15 minutes) much longer than US children (median time: 3.66 minutes).

But, US children waited to open the gift (median wait time: 14.54 minutes) longer than Japanese children (median time: 4.62 minutes).

When you look at the graphs, you’ll be impressed by the precise degree to which cultural expectations reverse wait times.

The Big Picture

So, what do we do with this information?

I think Yanaoka’s study offers us a specific reminder, and a general reminder.

Specificallythis study lets us know that self-control is NOT one monolithic, unchangeable thing.

Self-control varies across people and cultures. Yes, self-control matters; but, performance on one test — even a test with marshmallows — doesn’t tell us everything we need to know.

Generally, this study reminds us that culture always matters.

So, teachers should indeed welcome advice that experts offer us about — say — adolescence. But, that advice always includes cultural constraints. Adolescence, after all, differs in Denver, Kyoto, Sao Paolo, Reykjavik, and Gaborone.

So too cultural norms around stress. And feedback. And appropriate relationships between adults and students. Yes, and self-control.

No advice — not even research-based advice — gives us absolute guidance across all cultural norms.


Yanaoka, K., Michaelson, L. E., Guild, R. M., Dostart, G., Yonehiro, J., Saito, S., & Munakata, Y. (2022). Cultures crossing: the power of habit in delaying gratification. Psychological Science33(7), 1172-1181.

How Does Self-Control Really Work? Introducing a Debate
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Every teacher I know wishes that our students could control themselves just a little bit better. Or, occasionally, a whole lot better.

Rarely do we worry that students have too much self-control.

All these observations prompt us to ask: how does this thing called self-control really work?

In the field of psychology, that question has led to a fierce debate. If you’d like to enter into that debate, well, I’ve got some resources for you!

A Very Brief Introduction

Roy Baumeister has developed a well-known theory about self-control. You can read about it in depth in his book Willpower: Rediscovering the Greatest Human Strength, written with John Tierney.

Think of self-control as a kind of inner reservoir. My reservoir starts the day full. However, when I come down for breakfast, I see lots of bacon. I know I…MUST…RESIST…BACON, and that self-control effort drains my reservoir a bit.

However, once I finish my oatmeal and leave the kitchen, the bacon no longer tempts me so strongly. I’ve stopped draining the reservoir, and it can refill.

Baumeister’s theory focuses on all the things that drain the reservoir, and all the strategies we can use to a) refill it, or b) expand it.

Baumeister calls this process by a somewhat puzzling name: “ego depletion.” The “depletion” part makes good sense: my reservoir is depleted. The “ego” part isn’t as intuitive, but we’ll get used to that over time.

The key point: in recent years, the theory of ego depletion has come under debate — especially as part of the larger “replication crisis” in psychology.

Some say the theory has (literally) hundreds of studies supporting it. Others note methodological problems, and worry that non-replications languish in file drawers.

Welcome Aboard

Because self-control is so important to teachers, you just might be intrigued and want to learn more.

One great resource is a podcast, charmingly titled “Two Psychologists, Four Beers.” A couple times a month, Yoel Inbar and Michael Inzlicht get together over a few brews and chat about a topic.

In this episode, they talk about this controversy at length and in detail. SO MUCH interesting and helpful information here.

One key point to know: Inzlicht himself is a key doubter of Baumeister’s research. He’s not a dispassionate observer, but an important critic.

Friendly On Ramp

However interested you are in the topic of self-control, you might not have 80 minutes to devote to it.

Or, you might worry it will be overly complex to understand the first time through.

Good news! Ahmad Assinnari has put together a point-by-point summary of the podcast. 

You could read it as an introduction to an upcoming debate, and/or follow along to be sure you’re tracking the argument clearly. (BTW: Assinnari refers to Inzicht both as “Inzlicht” and as “Michael.” And, beware: it’s easy to confuse “Michael” with “Michel,” another scholar in the field.)

So, if you’d like to learn more, but you’re not sure you want to read Baumeister’s book, this post serves as an introduction to Assinnari’s summary. And, Assinnari’s summary introduces the podcast.

With these few steps, you’ll be up to speed on a very important debate.

How Best to Achieve our New Year’s Resolutions
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

When teachers seek out guidance to improve our teaching, we can turn to psychology and neuroscience research for inspiration.

When we just want to accomplish — at last!! — our New Year’s resolutions, can we find help here as well?

Turns out: we can!

Building Virtuous Habits

Several years ago, Charles Duhigg wrote a wonderful book called The Power of Habit.

We often think of habits as mere routines that dull our lives. But, if we can create good habits, we can be much more productive — in whatever way we define productive.

Duhigg created a handy flow chart to help explain the habit-building process: cues, rewards, and routines. There’s LOTS of science behind the recommendations, and you should look at his book if you want to know more.

If you just want to jump-start your new-year’s habit right away, you can check out that flow chart here.

The Self-Control Reservoir

To achieve our new year’s resolutions, we need to use self-control. So: what’s the best way to do that?

The first step, of course, is to understand: what kind of thing is self-control?

Back in the day, psychologists thought it was a personality trait. Some people are basically patient; some impatient. Some are basically conscientious. Others, not so much.

So, psychologists thought of self-control that way.

Over the years, Roy Baumeister has persuaded many folks to rethink that approach. In his view, self-control is more of a reservoir.

That is: I start with a certain amount of self-control. When I start using it — say, by resisting a second helping of cake — I drain my reservoir a bit.

If my host keeps offering me cake, and I keep saying “no thank you,” I drain it further and further. At some point, if the reservoir gets low enough, I won’t be able to maintain self-control.

Either I’ll accept that second slice, or I’ll say something rude about the badgering. After all, it took lots of self-control to maintain my temper.

We probably all start with somewhat different reservoirs of self-control. But, whatever reservoir we’ve got, it drains as we resist temptation. And then refills when temptation goes away.

Maintaining the Reservoir

If self-control is a reservoir, then we need to use it sparingly.

As Baumeister explains in his book Willpower, our typical New Year’s Resolution strategy runs directly counter to this understanding.

If I’ve got five resolutions, and I commit to all of them at once, I’m likely to drain the reservoir too much and too fast.

Instead, I should prioritize. I’ll pick the most important of the five, and use a bit of my self-control reservoir to building this new virtuous habit. (See Duhigg above.)

Only after I’ve created that new habit should I get to work on the second resolution. And, once that has become habit, I’ll turn my attention to the third.

By 1) spreading out this self-control effort, and 2) consciously building virtuous habits, I’ve got a much better chance of meeting my goals for this year.

And: no, thank you, no more cake for me.


I should say that, as is so often the case, the research cited by Duhigg and Baumeister does include some controversies. Not everyone is persuaded. For the time being, however, I think these theories hold up well, and are the best we’ve got.

Powerful Evidence: Self-Control Training Works — and Changes Brains
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Whenever we put a lot of effort into a project, we really want to believe that it helped.

For that reason, we might somehow overlook the signs that our efforts fell short. Or, we might exaggerate skimpy data to suggest that we succeeded.

To overcome these all-too-human tendencies, we need well designed research to gather and analyze data. The only way to demonstrate success is to look hard for failure.

Taking It Up a Notch

To look especially hard for failure, we might look at two different kinds of evidence.

For example: does self-control training work?

To answer this question, let’s start by having 11-year-olds go through a self-control training program. At the same time, we’ll identify a control group that doesn’t get the training.

We can see if the training worked in two different ways.

First: several years later, have those children (now 25 years old!) provide information about their lives. Have they completed high school? College? Do they have a job? Have they been arrested? Do they frequently get in fights?

We can also have their parents fill out similar surveys. Oh, and we’ll have the control group fill out surveys as well.

Are the children who got self-control training likelier to have more education and a job? Less likely to harm themselves and others? If yes, those differences suggests that they used those self-control strategies well.

Second: we can look at their brains.

In particular, we have decades of research showing the importance of a particular brain region for self control.

Roughly speaking, we want self-control regions of the brain — the prefrontal cortex (PFC) — to communicate well with the emotional drivers of human behavior. Better PFC communication means better self-control.

That brain region is in the middle (medial) part of the underside (ventral) of the PFC. So, we call it the ventromedial prefrontal cortex: vm-PFC. (Important note: neuroscience is fantastically complicated. This summary is a very streamlined version of a wildly intricate web of brain connectivity.)

So, after we survey the students who went through self-control training, we can have them hang out in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) gizmo.

Our hypothesis: trained students should have better vm-PFC connectivity between the PFC and brain regions that process emotions.

Today’s Research

A team of 14 researchers have in fact done all that.

An organization in rural Georgia called “Strong African American Families” wanted to improve the prospects of children living in poverty. They developed a program that included training for parents, and for their 11-year-old children.

Parents learned about “emotional support, [and] high levels of monitoring and control.”

The children “focused on forming goals for the future and making plans to attain them.” They also learned about strategies to use when encountering racism.

14 years later (!!), the researchers gathered both kinds of data described above. That is: the children (now 25) filled out surveys. And the had an fMRI scan to measure vm-PFC connectivity.

Sure enough, both measures suggested that the training made a real difference.

That is: the children who had the training did better on measures of adult self-control. And, they had higher levels of vm-PFC connectivity.

Reasonable Conclusions

The program run by Strong African American Families was tailored to the circumstances of its participants. We should not, in other words, conclude that their program will work for everyone.

But: we have quite persuasive evidence that their program had the effects it intended — poor children grew up as more responsible adults than un-trained peers.

And: we have a good neurobiological explanation for the different behavior — their altered life trajectory included developmental differences in the vm-PFC.

All these findings give us hope that well designed self-control programs can indeed have the effect that we want them to. That’s not just wishful thinking.

Can We Boost Our Students’ Self-Control?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

You have, no doubt, heard about this research before.

Walter Mischel tested preschoolers on self-control. In the famous “marshmallow test,” they got either one marshmallow right now, or two if they waited for fifteen minutes.

(I have to include an adorable video of children resisting marshmallows.)

Here’s the blockbuster part: the preschoolers’ performance on that test predicted their adult performance on similar self-control measures — four DECADES later.

And, as Roy Baumeister has shown, self-control influences … say … adult financial success. Or, likelihood of addiction. Or, even the odds that I’ll wind up in jail.

These paragraphs add up to a scary story. If self-control a) can be predicted in early childhood, and b) meaningfully shapes core adult behaviors and abilities, then we might worry about an individual’s capacity to determine his or her life’s direction.

And, we might particularly worry about a teacher’s ability to provide meaningful long-term help.

Ugh.

Bad News/Good News

A recent study sheds new light on this debate.

Here’s the headline: Y.E. Willems and others ran a meta-analysis on the heritability of self-control. Looking at 31 twin studies that included over 30,000 individuals, they conclude that overall heritabililty of self-control is 60%.

But what, precisely, does that mean?

For two reasons, I think this finding defeats the “scary story” I told a few paragraphs ago.

First reason: however you interpret “heritability,” we see that plenty of self-control isn’t determined by it. And, if self-control isn’t fully heritable, then the environment can influence it.

Who helps create environment? Teachers do.

In other words: all those self-control strategies we’ve been blogging about aren’t foolishly trying to defy genetic destiny. Instead, they’ve got plenty of room to work in.

“Heritabililty” Isn’t (At ALL) What We Think It Is

The second reason this research can calm our fears about the scary story gets technical.

“Heritability” sounds like it answers this question: “how much of a particular trait is determined by genes?”

In other words: how much does genetic variety explain dyslexia? Or, propensity for violence? Or, working memory capacity?

That’s not what heritabililty means.

Instead, heritabililty measures the amount of variation in a particular trait explained by genes.

This difference takes a long time to explain. Happily, we’ve got an expert ready to explain it.

Here’s Robert Sapolsky: Stanford professor (and 3-time Learning and the Brain speaker):

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=OareDiaR0hg

As I said, this kind of analysis can be tricky to follow. But the core message is crucial:

Environment matters for the development of self-control.

Yes, of course, genes have an influence on our self-control. But, so does the environment in which those genes create proteins, which — after a staggeringly complex process — influence behavior.

All those self-control boosting techniques you’ve been hearing about at Learning and the Brain conferences: you can have confidence. They might not change everything overnight.

But, they can indeed help. And, studies about heritability don’t mean what we think they do, so they shouldn’t discourage us from trying.


By the way, Sapolsky’s book Behave goes into this topic with clarity, humor, and precision. If you want to understand the nuances of genetic and environmental interactions, it’s a splendid read.

 

 

Healthy Snacks After Exercise? Depends on the Timing…
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

If you saw Roy Baumeister at the 2015 Learning and the Brain conference in Boston, you remember his presentation on self-control.

Of course, teachers care A LOT about self-control.

We need our students to control their behavior. (“Do not use the bunsen burner to light your backpack on fire,” my 6th grade science teacher said to me. Often.)

And, we need them to control their cognitive processes. (“When balancing chemical equations, start by identifying the elements.”)

Baumeister found, among other fascinating things, that both kinds of self-control “drain the same reservoir.”

That is: if I use up some self-control resisting the temptation to climb the jungle gym, I have less self-control left over to process the steps involved in subtracting two-digit numbers. (Baumeister’s book Willpower, written with John Tierney, explains his research in helpful detail.)

Replication Controversy

As the field of psychology wrestles with the “replication crisis,” Baumeister’s conclusions have come under question.

Some researchers haven’t gotten the same results when they run self-control experiments. Some question the research field in general. (For instance: terms like “self-control” are notoriously hard to define.)

This question matters to us. If Baumeister’s theories don’t hold water, then it’s unlikely the self-control solutions he proposes will help very much.

So, to take only the most recent example, John Medina’s Attack of the Teenage Brain devotes several chapters to helping adolescents develop executive functions — such as self-control.

If the research that Medina cites can’t be trusted…we might be back to square one.

Latest News

I’ve just found some pertinent research in an unlikely field: exercise and nutrition.

Researcher Christopher Gustafson and Co. asked visitors at a local gym to wear an accelerometer, purportedly so they could “keep track of relevant exercise data.” As a reward for participating, they were given a free snack after their workout.

In fact, the “accelerometer data” story masked the real interest of the study: participants’ snack choice.

All participants chose between a brownie and an apple. Some got the choice before they exercised; some after. Did the timing of the choice matter?

If Baumeister’s theory holds up, we would expect a difference between these two groups. Here’s why…

Self-Control, Snacks, and Exercise

Because apples are a healthier snack than brownies, we know we ought to choose them. But, for most of us, brownies taste a lot better. And so, we must use self-control to make that choice.

Likewise, we know that exercise is good for us. But, we rarely want to do it — and so that choice also takes self-control.

If I make the snack choice before exercise, my self-control reservoir remains relatively full. As a result, I’m likelier to make the “right” choice.

But, if I select my snack as I towel off after exercise, I’ve probably drained that reservoir considerably. So, I’ve got less willpower left. And I’m likelier to give into chocolatey temptation.

Is that what Gustafson found? Indeed he did.

In fact, 17% fewer people chose the apple, and 6% more chose the brownie. (The rest turned down a snack altogether.)

In other words: this study supports Baumeister, and gives us increased confidence in the research suggestions that flow from it.

What are some of those suggestions? You can start with an intriguing one here.

The Self-Control Paradox: Resistance is (Often) Futile
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Oscar Wilde famously said that he could resist everything but temptation.

This may be the only way that I’m a lot like Wilde. You, too, probably resemble this great Irish wit.

self-control paradox

Misunderstanding Self-Control

Self-control seems like a straightforward concept.

In front of me is a slice of chocolate cake. Or pizza. Or the very latest tech gizmo.

I really want it. Like, a lot.

But I steel myself and, like Odysseus resisting the Sirens, I deny myself this treat. That’s self-control.

Much of the research into self-control resembles this scenario. Roy Baumeister, one of the best-known self-control researchers, often asked study participants to resist chocolate chip cookies.

If you can resist freshly-baked chocolate chip cookies…well…that’s self-control.

Clearly, if we want to helps our students with self-control, we should help them resist such temptations.

The Self-Control Paradox

Or, maybe not.

Maybe you, and me, and Oscar Wilde are all really bad at resisting temptation. Maybe that’s not how self-control works at all.

Instead, in one of the most interesting psychology studies I’ve read, several researchers come to a different conclusion.

People who successfully resist temptation don’t stare down the chocolate chip cookies and boldly exert their self-control.

Instead, they use their self-control to avoid temptation in the first place.

That’s right: effective self-control isn’t exactly self-control. Instead, people who score highest on measures of self-control use it to develop virtuous habits and avoid tempting situations.

The reason I’m good at, say, staying on my diet is NOT that I resist cupcakes.

Instead, I’m good at dieting because a) I have gotten in the habit of making a salad, and b) my lunchtime walk never approaches the pizza joint.

I never have to resist temptation, because I use self-control to avoid temptation in the first place.

Odysseus, Reconsidered

Come to think of it: Odysseus didn’t use his self-control to resist the Sirens. He was, after all, tied to the mast at the time he sailed past them.

Instead, he used his self-control to prepare for temptation. Because he knew he couldn’t resist it, he made a plan to ensure the temptation wouldn’t lead him astray.

If we want our students to improve their executive functioning, if we want them to get better at self-control, then we should not focus on resisting temptation.

Instead, we should focus on avoiding temptation.

Resistance might be futile. But: the self-control paradox suggests we can bypass resistance altogether.

 

Adolescents and Self-Control: Do Teens Recognize High Stakes?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Anyone who works with teenagers — teachers and parents — wonders about the mystery of adolescent self-control.

At times, they prove capable of magnificent cognitive accomplishment.Adolescent self-control

(A high-school junior I taught once composed a new soliloquy for Hamlet. Speaking of Claudius — the uncle who murdered Hamlet’s own father — Hamlet says: “My unfather unfathered me.” I think the Bard himself envies that line.)

And, at other times, they baffle us with their extraordinary foolishness.

(At the next Learning and the Brain conference, ask me about the teens who kidnapped a teacher’s dog as a gesture of respect and affection.)

How do we make sense of this puzzle?

Adolescent Self-Control: High Stakes and Mistakes

Recent research offers one intriguing answer.

Catherine Insel, working as part of Leah Somerville’s lab, wondered if teens recognize the difference between high stakes and low stakes. Better said: she wanted to know if they behaved differently in those distinct settings.

She had students aged 13-20 perform a “go/no-go task.” When they saw a blue circle or a yellow circle or a purpley circle, they pressed a button. When they saw a stripey circle, they did NOT press the button. That is, they had to inhibit the instinct to press the button.

That’s a kind of self-control.

Some of the time, they faced small rewards and penalties: plus twenty cents for getting it right, minus ten cents for getting it wrong.

Some of the time, they faced larger rewards and penalties: plus one dollar for getting it right, minutes fifty cents for getting it wrong.

You might predict that adolescents would be more careful when the stakes were higher. That is, their score would be better when a WHOLE DOLLAR was on the line.

But: nope. That’s not what happened.

In the age groups from 13-18, they did equally well on low- and high-stakes tasks. Only the 19- and 20-year-olds were measurably better at high-stakes than low-stakes.

Put simply: adolescents simply didn’t respond to the difference between high-stakes and low-stakes tests.

Adolescent Self-control: The Brain Part

So far, Insel and colleagues were looking at behavior; that’s the study of psychology. They also looked at brain differences; that’s the study of neuroscience.

In particular, they focused on two brain areas.

The pre-frontal cortex — the part of the brain just behind the forehead — helps manage “higher” brain processes, such as inhibition.

The striatum — deep in the center of the brain — is a key part of the “reward network,” and influences motivation and decision-making.

(By the way, almost ALL brain regions — including the pre-frontal cortex and the striatum — participate in MANY different brain functions.)

They found that the connection between these regions matures over time.

That is, the self-control functions of the pre-frontal cortex are increasingly able to manage the reward networks of the striatum.

No wonder, then, that adolescents get better at controlling their impulses. Only gradually does the “control” part of the brain take firm control over the “impulse” part of the brain.

Teaching Implications

Insel’s research shows not only THAT teens don’t effectively distinguish between high- and low-stakes; it helps explain WHY they don’t: the appropriate brain networks haven’t fully matured.

This research suggests that high-stakes testing just might not be developmentally appropriate for this age group.

After all: adults recognize the importance of high-stakes work. We know to prepare for job interviews differently than we do for daily meetings. We know to be on our best behavior when we meet potential future in-laws; perhaps we relax a bit once they’re actual in-laws.

Teens, however, just don’t recognize that distinction as well.

In other words: if you needed another reason to downplay high-stakes testing, Insel and Somerville’s research provides just that.

More to Know

If you’re particularly interesting in this topic, we’ve posted about it frequently on this blog.

Here’s a link to Somerville’s work, in which she explores the boundaries between adolescence ad adulthood.

Here’s a Ted-talk by Sarah-Jayne Blakemore exploring the mysteries of adolescence.

Richard Cash is running an LatB Workshop specifically on self-regulation. You can check it out here. And, I’m running a Learning and the Brain workshop on teaching adolescents in April. Click here if you’re interested in learning more.

 

Bilingual Preschoolers and Self-Control
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

AdobeStock_124990522 [Converted]_Credit

If you can speak two or more languages, you’re likely to have some real advantages in life. For starters, you can talk easily with lots more people, and turn off the subtitles on more movies.

Are there cognitive benefits to bilingualism? That is, does being bilingual help you think better?

(more…)