classroom advice – Page 16 – Education & Teacher Conferences Skip to main content
Adolescents and Self-Control: Do Teens Recognize High Stakes?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Anyone who works with teenagers — teachers and parents — wonders about the mystery of adolescent self-control.

At times, they prove capable of magnificent cognitive accomplishment.Adolescent self-control

(A high-school junior I taught once composed a new soliloquy for Hamlet. Speaking of Claudius — the uncle who murdered Hamlet’s own father — Hamlet says: “My unfather unfathered me.” I think the Bard himself envies that line.)

And, at other times, they baffle us with their extraordinary foolishness.

(At the next Learning and the Brain conference, ask me about the teens who kidnapped a teacher’s dog as a gesture of respect and affection.)

How do we make sense of this puzzle?

Adolescent Self-Control: High Stakes and Mistakes

Recent research offers one intriguing answer.

Catherine Insel, working as part of Leah Somerville’s lab, wondered if teens recognize the difference between high stakes and low stakes. Better said: she wanted to know if they behaved differently in those distinct settings.

She had students aged 13-20 perform a “go/no-go task.” When they saw a blue circle or a yellow circle or a purpley circle, they pressed a button. When they saw a stripey circle, they did NOT press the button. That is, they had to inhibit the instinct to press the button.

That’s a kind of self-control.

Some of the time, they faced small rewards and penalties: plus twenty cents for getting it right, minus ten cents for getting it wrong.

Some of the time, they faced larger rewards and penalties: plus one dollar for getting it right, minutes fifty cents for getting it wrong.

You might predict that adolescents would be more careful when the stakes were higher. That is, their score would be better when a WHOLE DOLLAR was on the line.

But: nope. That’s not what happened.

In the age groups from 13-18, they did equally well on low- and high-stakes tasks. Only the 19- and 20-year-olds were measurably better at high-stakes than low-stakes.

Put simply: adolescents simply didn’t respond to the difference between high-stakes and low-stakes tests.

Adolescent Self-control: The Brain Part

So far, Insel and colleagues were looking at behavior; that’s the study of psychology. They also looked at brain differences; that’s the study of neuroscience.

In particular, they focused on two brain areas.

The pre-frontal cortex — the part of the brain just behind the forehead — helps manage “higher” brain processes, such as inhibition.

The striatum — deep in the center of the brain — is a key part of the “reward network,” and influences motivation and decision-making.

(By the way, almost ALL brain regions — including the pre-frontal cortex and the striatum — participate in MANY different brain functions.)

They found that the connection between these regions matures over time.

That is, the self-control functions of the pre-frontal cortex are increasingly able to manage the reward networks of the striatum.

No wonder, then, that adolescents get better at controlling their impulses. Only gradually does the “control” part of the brain take firm control over the “impulse” part of the brain.

Teaching Implications

Insel’s research shows not only THAT teens don’t effectively distinguish between high- and low-stakes; it helps explain WHY they don’t: the appropriate brain networks haven’t fully matured.

This research suggests that high-stakes testing just might not be developmentally appropriate for this age group.

After all: adults recognize the importance of high-stakes work. We know to prepare for job interviews differently than we do for daily meetings. We know to be on our best behavior when we meet potential future in-laws; perhaps we relax a bit once they’re actual in-laws.

Teens, however, just don’t recognize that distinction as well.

In other words: if you needed another reason to downplay high-stakes testing, Insel and Somerville’s research provides just that.

More to Know

If you’re particularly interesting in this topic, we’ve posted about it frequently on this blog.

Here’s a link to Somerville’s work, in which she explores the boundaries between adolescence ad adulthood.

Here’s a Ted-talk by Sarah-Jayne Blakemore exploring the mysteries of adolescence.

Richard Cash is running an LatB Workshop specifically on self-regulation. You can check it out here. And, I’m running a Learning and the Brain workshop on teaching adolescents in April. Click here if you’re interested in learning more.

 

Point/Counterpoint: Escaping the Inquiry Learning Debate
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Several days ago, I posted some thoughts about the benefits of Direct Instruction. That post specifically contrasted the benefits of DI with the perils Inquiry Learning. Specifically, Hattie finds Inquiry Learning to be largely ineffective.assessing inquiry learning

The Learning Scientists have also published some skeptical thoughts about Inquiry Learning. In their most recent weekly digest, to promote balance, they offer links to some pro-Inquiry-Learning counter-arguments. If you’re an IL skeptic, you might want to check them out.

Assessing Inquiry Learning: What’s a Teacher to Do?

When we face conflicting evidence about any particular pedagogy, teachers can always focus instead on specific cognitive capacities.

For example: working memory.

If an Inquiry Learning lesson plan ramps working memory demands up too steeply, then students probably won’t learn very much.

Of course: if a Direct Instruction lesson plan ramps up WM demands, then those students won’t learn very much either.

The key variable — in this analysis — is not the specifics of the pedagogical approach. Instead, teachers can focus on the match between our teaching and the cognitive apparatus that allows learning.

In other words: overwhelming working memory is ALWAYS bad — it doesn’t matter if your lesson plan is DI or IL.

The same point can be made for other cognitive capacities.

Lesson plans that disorient students — that is, ones that interfere with attention — will hamper learning. So too motivation. So too stress.

When assessing Inquiry Learning, don’t ask yourself “does my lesson plan fit this pedagogical theory perfectly?” Ask yourself: “does my lesson plan realistically align with my students’ cognitive systems?

The answer to that question will give you the wisest guidance.

Despite the Skeptics, a Champion of Direct Instruction
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

In the debates between “progressive” and “traditional” educational theories, few arguments rage hotter than the battle between project based learning and direct instruction.

PBL’s proponents take a constructivist perspective. They argue that people learn by building their own meaning from discrete units of information.benefits of direct instruction

In this view, teachers can’t simply download conclusions into students’ brains. We can’t, that is, just tell students the right answer.

Instead, we should let them wrestle with complexities and come to their own enduring understanding of the material they’re learning.

An Alternative Perspective: The Benefits of Direct Instruction

In a recent meta-analysis, Jean Stockard’s team argues that direct instruction clearly works.

Looking at 300+ studies from over 50 years, they conclude that DI benefits students in every grade, in a variety of racial and ethnic groups, with a variety of learning differences, from every socio-economic background.

Of course, this research conclusion challenges some often-repeated assurances that direct instruction simply can’t help students learn.

(The recent meta-analysis is, unfortunately, behind a paywall. You can, however, see some impressive graphs in an earlier white paper by Stockard.)

Another Alternative Perspective: Reinterpreting “Constructivism”

Interestingly, Stockard doesn’t disagree with a constructivist understanding of learning. Instead, she sees direct instruction as a kind of constructivism.

“DI shares with constuctivism the important basic understanding that students interpret and make sense of information with which they are presented. The difference lies in the nature of the information given to students, with DI theorists stressing the importance of very carefully choosing and structuring examples so they are as clear and unambiguous as possible.”

(This quotation comes from a brief pre-publication excerpt of the meta-analysis, which you can find here.)

In other words: in Stockard’s view, the difference between PBL and DI isn’t that one is constructivist and the other isn’t.

Instead, these theories disagree about the kind of information that allows students to learn most effectively.

Simply put: PBL theorists think that relatively more, relatively unstructured information helps students in their mental building projects. DI theorists think that relatively less, relatively tightly structured information benefits students.

Stockard makes her own views quite plain:

“It is clear that students make sense of and interpret the information that they are given–but that their learning is enhanced only when the information presented is explicit, logically organized, and clearly sequenced. To do anything less shirks the responsibility of effective instruction.”

You might mentally add a “mic drop” at the end of that passage.

Other Sources

Of course, lots of people write on this topic.

John Hattie’s meta-meta-analyses have shown DI to be quite effective. This Hattie website, for example, shows an effect size of 0.60. (For Problem based learning, it’s 0.12; for Inquiry based teaching, it’s 0.35.)

If you like a feisty blogger on this topic, Greg Ashman consistently champions direct instruction.

And, I’ve written about the difficulties of measuring PBL’s success here.

Surprise! The Unexpected Outdoor Class Advantage
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

outdoor learning advantage

“Can we have class outside today?”

If you’re like me, you get this question often. Especially on a beautiful spring day…

But do your students have a point? Might there be good reasons to move class outside every now and then?

Outdoor Class Advantage: What We Know

We’ve already got research suggesting that your students might be on to something.

Some researchers suggest that classes outside help restore student attention.

Other studies (here and here) indicated that they might enhance student motivation as well.

We’ve even got reason to think that exposure to green landscape helps students learn. For example: this study in Michigan suggests that natural views improve graduation rate and standardized test scores.

None of the evidence is completely persuasive, but each additional piece makes the argument even stronger.

Outdoor Class Advantage: Today’s News

If I’m a skeptic about outdoor class, I might make the following argument. Outdoor classes might be good for that particular class. However, they might be bad for subsequent classes.

That is: students might be so amped up by their time outside that they can’t focus when they get back indoors.

To explore this concern, Ming Kuo and colleagues put together an impressive study.

Over ten weeks, two teachers taught several pairs of lessons. Half of the time, the first lesson was taught outside. For the other half, the first lesson was taught inside.

Researchers then measured students’ attentiveness during the second lesson in these pairs.

The results?

The Results!

Students were more attentive — A LOT more attentive — after outdoor classes than indoor classes.

In almost 50% of the lessons, attention was a full standard deviation higher after outdoor classes. In 20% of the lessons, it was two standard deviations higher.

Technically speaking, that difference is HUGE.

(By the way: the researchers came up with several different ways to measure attention. Outdoor classes led to improved attention in four of the five measures.)

The Implications

This research suggests that teachers needn’t worry about outdoor classes leading to distraction in subsequent classes.

That finding doesn’t necessarily mean that outdoor classes benefit learning, but it does mean we have fewer potential causes for concern.

Getting the Best Advice about Learning
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Occasionally I try to persuade people that neuroscience is fantastically complicated. In other words: we shouldn’t beat ourselves up if we don’t master it all.

Today I spotted a headline that makes my point for me:

 

Hippocampus-driven feed-forward inhibition of the prefrontal cortex mediates relapse of extinguished fear

Got that?

What’s the Bigger Point?

Neuroscience is simply fascinating. As teachers, we really want to know how neurons work. And synapses. And brain regions — like the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex.

However, specific teaching advice almost always comes from psychology. How do teachers help students connect neurons to create memories? Psychology. What classroom strategies support executive function in the prefrontal cortex? Psychology.

At a LatB Conference, you’ll enjoy the neuroscience talks because they show you what’s going on underneath the hood. At the psychology talks, you’ll get specific classroom suggestions.

The best conference experience, in my opinion, combines both.

Motivating Retrieval Practice: Money Doesn’t Help
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Brain Chalkboard_CreditGiven all the benefits that come from retrieval practice, we should surely encourage our students to use this technique as much as possible. How can we best motivate them to do so?

Three researchers in Europe offer this answer: subtly.

More specifically, their research finds that offering students extrinsic rewards for their retrieval practice reduced its effectiveness.

Students offered rewards made more mistakes when they first tried to recall information, and–even taking those initial errors into account–remembered less than their fellow students who had received no enticement to practice.

In this study, the extrinsic rewards were cash payments: students received a euro for every correct answer. In schools, we rarely pay students money to get correct answers. However, we quite often pay them with grades.

This study suggests that retrieval practice should–as much as possible–come in the form of very-low-stakes or no-stakes retrieval.

Can Meaningful Gestures Help STEM Students Learn Better?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Learning STEM with Gestures

As schools focus more on STEM disciplines, teachers strive to help our students master complex STEM concepts.

After all, it’s hard enough to say “magnetic anisotrophy,” much less understand what it is.

Researchers Dane DeSutter and Mike Stieff have several suggestions for teachers. Specifically, they argue that spatial thinking–essential to many STEM concepts–can be enhanced by appropriate gestures.

(more…)

Improving the Syllabus: Surprising Benefits of Jumbling
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

AdobeStock_119098880_Credit

Recent entries on this blog have focused on the kind of practice that helps students learn best.

(Hint: it rhymes with “retrieval schmactrice.”)

What can researchers tell us about the schedule of that practice?

Imagine that my students are studying three different grammar topics: direct and indirect objects, predicate nominatives and predicate adjectives, and prepositional phrases. How should I organize the practice problems on the syllabus?

Jumbling practice problems?

I might put those practice problems in chunks: all the in/direct object questions, then all the PN and PA problems, and then the prep phrase problems. (Psychologists call this schedule “blocking,” because students are practicing in blocks.)

Or, I might jumble all the practice problems together: a prep phrase question followed by an indirect object question followed by a predicate adjective problem. (The technical term here is “interleaving.”)

Which schedule works better?

And, does that schedule help both factual learning (grammar) and motor learning (tennis)?

This brief video, starring Bob Bjork, has the answers:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?time_continue=18&v=l-1K61BalIA

As a bonus, here’s a study where a college professor tried to interleave material in her classroom.

When Homework Is (and Isn’t) Genuinely Helpful
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Back in October, I posted a link to a pro-homework argument. Again today, I’ve stumbled across another–this one summarizing John Hattie’s Visible Learning on the subject.

Author Tom Sherrington makes two general points.

First: the question “does homework help students learn” is too broad. We need to narrow it down. What age student are we discussing? What kind of homework are they doing? What discipline are they studying?

This first point is often worth making. If someone asks you, “Is technology good for learning?” remember that the question is too big to answer sensibly. Likewise: “does gender matter for learning?” Or “can we train brains?”

Research can answer narrow question very well. The bigger the question, the less certain our answer.

Second: the brief answer to the question is: homework is helpful for older students, but not for younger ones.

Of course, as outlined above, that brief answer requires lots of elaboration.

Benefiting from Retrieval Practice: Get the Timing Just Right
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Retrieval Practice Timing Affects its Benefits

I’ve posted a lot here recently about retrieval practice: the practice of reviewing material by pulling it OUT of the student’s head rather than trying to put it back IN.

For example: if I ask my students to write down the 5 main points from yesterday’s class about the Buddha, that’s retrieval practice. They have to get info out of their heads.

If, instead, I remind them of the 5 main points from yesterday’s class, that’s not retrieval practice. After all, I’m putting information back in.

The short sales pitch for retrieval practice is: it works for all students in all subjects, all the time. (Ask Dr. Pooja Agarwal.)

Unless…

Of course, all students in all subjects all the time is quite a grand claim. It’s rare for any teaching practice to work all the time, so we should be on the lookout for boundary conditions.

And, indeed, one has recently jumped out at me.

The story is interestingly complicated. I promise, however, that a close study of this complexity leads to specific and useful teaching advice. So: hang in there!

When Retrieval Practice Timing Might Be Bad

Imagine that, in yesterday’s class, we went over ten definitions for key economics terms. I want to begin today’s class with a quick review, so we go back over five of those terms.

My assumption is that, by reviewing five, I’m actually helping you to remember all ten.

Here’s the surprising research finding: by practicing some of the terms, I actually make it LESS LIKELY that you’ll remember the unpracticed terms.

In other words: recalling some of the words prompts you to forget the unpracticed words.

Psychologists call this bizarre result retrieval-induced forgetting. After all, the retrieval — that is, the practice — induced you to forget.

When Research Fields Contradict

So: the retrieval practice research says that retrieval is beneficial for memory.

And: the retrieval-induced forgetting research says that retrieval is detrimental for memory.

What happens when teachers do both? Does one cancel out the other? Can Superman defeat Iron Man?

Research done by Jason CK Chan helps answer this intriguing question.

The short answer is: in the short term, retrieval-induced forgetting is stronger. So: if I quiz you on five of those economics terms, and then give you the final test on those terms an hour later, you’re more likely to forget the five unpracticed words.

However, in the longer term, retrieval practice is stronger. So: the quiz on five terms will benefit you if you take that final test 24 hours later.

This result is especially likely if my quiz encourages you to think about how these five words connect conceptually to the other words.

Practical Advice

Although these research findings can be difficult to follow, they do all lead to a specific suggestion.

Retrieval practice is an excellent study strategy for students more than 24 hours ahead of a test. However, within that 24 hour window, teachers and students should focus more on connecting ideas rather than recalling them.

To update Dr. Agarwal’s guidance: retrieval practice works for all students in all subjects, (almost) all the time.