Skip to main content
The Struggles of Young-for-their-Grade Students
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Relatively young students

Several years ago I taught Jacob: an affable high school sophomore notable for his quick wit, his impressive height…and his immaturity. He was, technically speaking, goofy. Jacob’s peers noticed, and didn’t appreciate his antics. (Neither did I.)

When I met his parents for a teacher conference, I commented on his surprisingly juvenile behavior. They exchanged glances, and his mother said: “Well, he is the youngest student in the sophomore class. He could be a freshman.”

This news made all the difference to me. I had been fooled by Jacob’s 6′ 2″ frame. His behavior, odd for a 10th grader, was entirely appropriate for a 9th grader. When I started giving the structure he needed, he calmed down. And grew up.

By the end of the year, he worked with his classmates very effectively.

The Travails of Relatively Young Students…

A recent BrainBlogger post describes the Jacobs of the educational world. If a school has a strict cut-off date for a particular grade, then some students will be almost a full year younger than others.

In college, this difference shouldn’t matter much. After all, 19-year-olds and 20-year-olds should be emotionally and cognitively well matched.

In younger grades, however, that age difference can be huge. The age-appropriate developmental differences between the youngest and the oldest kindergartener might be substantial.

BrainBlogger’s author–identified only by her first name Naomi–outlines the alarming and ongoing consequences of this early developmental gap.

  • Relatively young students are likelier to be criticized for their immaturity–as happened with my student Jacob.
  • They are likelier to be diagnosed with ADHD.
  • Relatively older students are likelier to be accepted into Gifted programs, even if they’re not gifted.
  • Relatively young students are less likely to take the high-stakes exams that shape educational possibilities in some countries.
  • They are less likely to attend college, and also less likely to graduate from college.

…and, some benefits

At the same time, Naomi is careful to note the complexity of the question.

In the first place, as she writes, “the impact of [relative age effects] on educational attainment is…probabilistic not deterministic.” That is, some younger students will do just fine, even if their group is less likely to do so.

In fact, some research shows the advantages of being at the younger end of a grade’s age spectrum. For instance, younger students get the message that they need to work harder to succeed as much as their older peers, and so might have a better work ethic.

Next Steps

If you’d like to think more about this complex question, I’d start by looking over Naomi’s article. She lays out the research well, and includes sources from many different countries.

In the meanwhile, you’ve now got a helpful new question to ask. When working with students whose behavior makes you wonder about ADHD, you might start by looking up their age.

 

Can Meaningful Gestures Help STEM Students Learn Better?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Learning STEM with Gestures

As schools focus more on STEM disciplines, teachers strive to help our students master complex STEM concepts.

After all, it’s hard enough to say “magnetic anisotrophy,” much less understand what it is.

Researchers Dane DeSutter and Mike Stieff have several suggestions for teachers. Specifically, they argue that spatial thinking–essential to many STEM concepts–can be enhanced by appropriate gestures.

(more…)

Does Forest-Bathing Benefit Your Anxious Amygdala?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

AdobeStock_94176737

You have perhaps heard of “forest-bathing,” the Japanese practice of taking in the forest atmosphere to boost health.

For many, the idea has intrinsic appeal. (I work at a summer camp in leafy Vermont, and so am immediately drawn to ideas like these.)

Do we see any neural changes as a result of time spent in the forest?

The short answer: living near forests helps

According to a recent study looking at residents of Berlin, the answer is “yes.”

Those who live in or near forests demonstrate more “amygdala integrity” than those who don’t. In fact, forest-living promotes healthy amygdala development even more than living near parks or other green spaces.

The study itself is quite technical, but the headline message is clear: the place where you live can influence brain development.

A Longer Answer: are we sure?

As is always true, we have many reasons to pause before we make dramatic changes in response to this study.

First, the authors conclude that living near forest promote “amygdala integrity,” but they don’t say what “amygdala integrity” means. It’s hard to be opposed to “integrity,” but I wish I knew more about this part of the finding.

Second, we should be cautious when evaluating research that supports our own biases. If you–like me–LOVE spending time in the forest, then you’ll be tempted to wave this study about to support your long-held convictions.

“See!” you might cry, “I’ve always told you that forests were good for you and [**whispering**] your amygdala integrity!”

Research that supports our own pet causes can often take advantage of our blindspots. We should be especially careful in promoting it.

Third, there’s an unfortunate history of people getting excited about “nature is really good for your brain” research.

The New York Times got very excited about a study trumpeting the benefits of walking through a forest, despite real concerns about methodology in that study.

And yet…

…despite these three reservations, I’m inclined to think that the researchers are on to something here. Living in an environment that mirrors our evolutionary heritage might very well be good for our brains’ development.

Improving the Syllabus: Surprising Benefits of Jumbling
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

AdobeStock_119098880_Credit

Recent entries on this blog have focused on the kind of practice that helps students learn best.

(Hint: it rhymes with “retrieval schmactrice.”)

What can researchers tell us about the schedule of that practice?

Imagine that my students are studying three different grammar topics: direct and indirect objects, predicate nominatives and predicate adjectives, and prepositional phrases. How should I organize the practice problems on the syllabus?

Jumbling practice problems?

I might put those practice problems in chunks: all the in/direct object questions, then all the PN and PA problems, and then the prep phrase problems. (Psychologists call this schedule “blocking,” because students are practicing in blocks.)

Or, I might jumble all the practice problems together: a prep phrase question followed by an indirect object question followed by a predicate adjective problem. (The technical term here is “interleaving.”)

Which schedule works better?

And, does that schedule help both factual learning (grammar) and motor learning (tennis)?

This brief video, starring Bob Bjork, has the answers:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?time_continue=18&v=l-1K61BalIA

As a bonus, here’s a study where a college professor tried to interleave material in her classroom.

When Homework Is (and Isn’t) Genuinely Helpful
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Back in October, I posted a link to a pro-homework argument. Again today, I’ve stumbled across another–this one summarizing John Hattie’s Visible Learning on the subject.

Author Tom Sherrington makes two general points.

First: the question “does homework help students learn” is too broad. We need to narrow it down. What age student are we discussing? What kind of homework are they doing? What discipline are they studying?

This first point is often worth making. If someone asks you, “Is technology good for learning?” remember that the question is too big to answer sensibly. Likewise: “does gender matter for learning?” Or “can we train brains?”

Research can answer narrow question very well. The bigger the question, the less certain our answer.

Second: the brief answer to the question is: homework is helpful for older students, but not for younger ones.

Of course, as outlined above, that brief answer requires lots of elaboration.

Default Image
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

I live in Boston. I’m a Tom Brady fan. But, good heavens, his Brain Training program looks like snake oil.

The website (and, no, I’m not providing a link) uses all the right buzzwords: “brain plasticity,” “personalize,” “money-back guarantee!”

Some of the claims have a surface plausibility. You can, in fact, train your ability to track objects in space. Video games can do that for you, too.

But the idea that all of this comes together to promote “brain speed” and “intelligence” seems laughable. (I don’t know what “brain speed” even means.)

The Recent History of “Brain Training”

Always remember: Lumosity was fined $2,000,000 for making false claims sounding like these. I suppose it’s possible that Brady’s Brain Team has cracked a code that no one else has. But, it seems mightily unlikely.

I’m so vexed that I’m tempted to make a joke about Deflate-gate. For a Patriots fan, that’s as bad as it gets.

Focus on the Speaker: Charles Fadel Champions Curriculum Redesign
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

https://vimeo.com/120748039

Charles Fadel will be speaking on “Creativity, Curiosity, and Collaboration” at the upcoming Learning & the Brain conference.

He is the founder of the Center for Curriculum Redesign at Harvard’s Graduate School of Education.

The Innovative Mission of the Center for Curriculum Redesign

Andrew Watson:

Most educational reform focuses on the way the teaching happens: project based learning, or flipped classrooms, or technology, or that sort of thing. Your focus is more on curriculum, which is to say, what it is that teachers are actually teaching. Why have you chosen that focus, instead of the method of teaching?

Charles Fadel:

When I was at Cisco, we started the Partnership for 21st Century Skills; we started looking at the skills that make people successful all their life.

But as I started getting more and more into this question, I realized—wow!—in the past 50 or so years everyone has been so focused on how to teach better, because somehow we assumed that what we taught was adequate. And it was probably adequate since the Industrial revolution, to a point. But nowadays, it’s really not adequate anymore.

There are several difficulties with this question.

First of all, if the curricula are already chock-full of knowledge items, there is no time and space available to teach skills, and character qualities, and meta-learning abilities. And systems are very, very resistant to removing some things so that there might be time and space for these new competencies.

CCRVennDiagramPrint

Second, there are all sort of human dynamics at play. Do you think that a literature expert will say, “Well, that’s fine, we can just read two books this year, and the other eight we’re just going to do cliff notes.” That’s never going to happen. Right?

Naturally enough, everyone considers that their discipline is the most important thing, and they’re not going to let go of it. We call this “expert (confirmation) bias.”

Third you have the politics. As I go around the world, I realize to what extent the curriculum is prisoner of parliaments and politicians. And they, for political expediency’s sake, decide some of the things that are taught. Whether it’s history, or languages, or so on—and these decisions also crowd the curriculum.

Funny enough, parliaments don’t have too many opinions about maths…

Andrew Watson:

People are generally in favor of math.

Charles Fadel:

Well, yes, we are too, but should that mean that we abdicate our responsibility entirely to the hands of maths academics? Who have no idea about how math is used in the real world.

So, for all of these reasons, I realized—wow!—the curriculum is in trouble. What should we be learning?

Do we need to learn the multiplication tables? Yes, no, and why?

Do we need to learn long division? Yes, no, and why?

How do we decide? And how can we remove obsolete and extraneous items to make time and space for

  1. deepening understanding of traditional disciplines (Maths, History etc.),
  2. adding modern branches to the traditional disciplines, like say adding algorithmics to mathematics, not just algebra and geometry,
  3. adding modern disciplines, like entrepreneurship, robotics, etc. and
  4. very importantly, combining all these disciplines with the competencies that need to be developed. By “competencies” I mean skills, character, and meta-learning.

How in the world are we going to do all this unless we curate what’s already on the dockets?

Andrew Watson:

How hard can that be?

Charles Fadel:

The current curriculum a bit like a budget deficit, you know. Every single line item you can justify in insolation from the rest. You know this restaurant you went to with your significant other, that was a good thing to do in isolation. But, you do this too many times and you’ve broken your budget.

So every single line item in the curriculum needs to be scrutinized, and that’s not something that disciplinary expert groups are particularly well suited to do left on their own—because of the “expert bias” I described before.

Andrew Watson:

I can tell you from my own school experience that the battles fought over curricular changes are quite fierce. So, it’s a good thing the Center for Curriculum Redesign exists.

Charles Fadel:

We at the Center are non-political and non-dogmatic. We don’t have an axe to grind; we can look at all these things more cold-bloodedly and say “you know what, after analyzing and polling, we haven’t heard a profound justification for why long division should stay, therefore it goes …”

Curriculum Redesign to Foster Creativity, Curiosity, Collaboration

Andrew Watson:

Okay. I’m going to change gears to introduce your talk at the up-coming Learning and the Brain conference. That talk, as I understand it, focuses on creativity, curiosity and collaboration, which are in the Skills portion of your book.

Charles Fadel:

Actually “creativity” and “collaboration” are Skills, but “curiosity” is in the Character portion.

Andrew Watson:

Oh right, okay. I want to think about those topics in reverse order.

Starting with collaboration: Is the way to teach collaboration just say, “Hey everybody, you have to work together to perform this task” and assume they’ll figure out how to collaborate? Or is it a skill we can teach with a collaboration curriculum? If “yes,” how do we do that?

Charles Fadel:

Any single one of these 12 competencies that we have identified in the book’s framework can be taught.

It’s not enough to say “Here, we’re going to throw you in a collaborative situation, good luck to you.”

The same way that there are better techniques for how to deploy projects, there are better techniques for how to collaborate. For instance, by understanding the psychological make up of your team members.

In the corporate world, for example, we use the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. I’m not saying that the Myers-Briggs has huge science behind it, but I’m saying that it’s a facilitator of the conversations of team members.

Andrew Watson:

Understood.

Charles Fadel:

The Myers-Briggs test might say, this person has a certain dominant personality characteristic, and that’s why they are not comfortable making decisions right away. Whereas I’ll be tempted to just jump to a conclusion and make a decision early on.

The test helps me take a deep breath about my own behavior, about their behavior. It forces certain metacognitive views of people and the world.

So that’s the sort of thing you can do to understand yourself and others better. That’s part of what you would want to learn to better collaborate.

You can also teach certain group principles, such as respect. Making sure that everyone has a chance to speak. Even that shy introvert in the corner. Make sure the team leader puts the spotlight on them and says, “you can you speak now, or if you’d be more comfortable you could send me an email.” Et cetera.

So, there are all sorts of techniques. To use communication analogy: the same way that they tell you “don’t wear a striped shirt when you go on camera,” you know there are plenty of tips and tricks that one needs to know to improve collaboration.

And so all of these things need to be taught. In fact, they should first be taught to teachers, because they have not necessarily been in a collaborative environment themselves. It’s such an isolated profession!

Andrew Watson:

Got it. Let’s talk a little bit about curiosity. Schools have a really bad reputation about curiosity and are often are accused of interfering with curiosity. Can you think of ways we would be better at promoting curiosity?

Charles Fadel:

Right, so first let’s have a balanced conversation about how schools are “bad at creativity.”

Remember, schools have to cover a certain amount of material in a certain period of time. Yes, we want to give students enough agency and free will to learn things their own way. But at the same time, there’s not an infinite time to allow them to experiment forever.

And so that’s the complexity of the balance to strike.

Now can traditional schooling do a far better job? Absolutely.

But I’m saying schools don’t have to be entirely one way or the other. Just as research shows that we need to have the right balance between didactic instruction and constructivism/projects, we also have to have the right balance of student choice and student non-choice.

Now, should we err in the direction of student choice right now? Yes, because we’ve gone too far in the wrong direction of non-choice.

With that said, there are ways to bring these approaches together.

For instance, let’s say that the curriculum says you’ve got to learn robotics. The choices could be: well I’m going to do a swimming robot, and you’re going to do a flying robot, then the third person is going to do an underwater robot. But Robotics are important so everyone has to learn that – their own way.

And you can have finer-grade choices as well.

Everyone does need to understand robotics, but we don’t all need to go beyond basic skills in programming.

If that is not where my forte is, I’m going to devote more of my time to making the robot emote and mimic emotion—because that’s what I’m attracted to. I want to go into anthropology later, so emotions are my interest.

So you see, there’s a blend of requirements and choices: meaning, even the anthropologist has to understand how to code. But, the anthropologist doesn’t have to be the best possible coder.

You’d say “okay anthropologist, you got passing grades in coding, move on. You don’t have to be the best one, but at least you understand the principles of coding, and now we can have you focus on the thing that jazzes you up – and know how to interface with the coder when needed later.”

So we recommend a constant blend of choice and prescription.

There’s an important graph we have in the book. The ratio between choice and prescription changes over time.

CF Prescription

When they’re young kids, most of the curriculum is going to be prescribed. We’re not going to let you decide whether or not you want to learn how to read and write and compute. It’s just not a choice. Otherwise you’re not going to be a functional member of modern society, your chances will be severely truncated…

But, by high school, it’s certainly clear that if you want to do a given type of project, you should have some latitude. And so the ratio of prescription to choice changes over time.

Andrew Watson:

In some ways it sounds like curiosity and collaboration work well together. Because I’m curious about anthropology, I’m going to collaborate with someone who has more technical skills in this robot building process. The fact that I’m working collaboratively in a team means I get the focus on the thing I’m most particularly curious about.

Charles Fadel:

Yes. These 12 competencies are not isolated; they’re not silos in our brains. All 12 interplay with each other.

Andrew Watson:

Let’s change gears and think about creativity. What strategies do you see out there that are going to be helpful in teaching students creativity?

Charles Fadel:

Remember that CCR focuses on the what—the curriculum—not the how—the teaching methods. If your questions relate to classroom techniques, I’m really not the right person to ask.

But, we have to be clear about the where. These competencies must be practiced in as many disciplines as possible.

We might be tempted to say: “arts teach creativity.” Well for creativity to stick – to transfer – it has to be activated in several different disciplines. We would want as many disciplines as possible to develop as many competencies as possible.

At the same time, let’s not fool ourselves. It may be a bit exaggerated to say that every discipline can help with every competency.

Andrew Watson:

So for all three of these—for collaboration, curiosity and creativity—you’re not at this point advocating teaching strategies. You’re making an argument that we have to teach these competencies, and you’re putting together a cognitive framework to help teachers think about how they inter-relate with each other.

Charles Fadel:

We’re trying to make sure that—at a minimum—each discipline is tasked or not tasked in developing them. Could you teach collaboration in every single discipline? Yes. Are some—like engineering and art—more conducive to that? Yes.

Part of the complexity is that teachers want to say: yes, I agree with you. Let’s all teach collaboration. However, perhaps some disciplines are better suited for some competencies than others. It’s exaggerated for every discipline to think that they’re going to do everything.

“Incremental Ambition”

Andrew Watson:

In the book, you describe your work as “incrementally ambitious,” which is a very fun phrasing. Can you unpack that a little bit?

Charles Fadel:

Sure. If you use a word like “radical,” it sounds good for getting on the New York Times front page. But, you know, that’s really not the point.

“Radical” implies you throw away the existing system. You “disrupt,” which is a term that is so over-used.

We want to profoundly change, but in an evolutionary, not revolutionary sort of way.

Now I hear the irritation of those who say, “to hell with the system. It’s not listening; therefore I’m going to take it down.”

But it’s a lot easier to take down than to build. And so the challenge for the CCR is change management.  That’s why we have to be ambitious, but incrementally so.

CF Pyramids

Andrew Watson:

The people who are at Learning and the Brain conferences, by definition, those are people who want to change what we’re doing. But, we don’t want the change to mean that everything we’ve ever worked for goes away. We want to be better at what we’re doing. Which is why the ambition has to be incremental.

Charles Fadel:

It’s a lot easier to hope for some magic solution, perhaps using technology, some extraordinary e-tutor. You just hope, oh if I only had a magic wand. That’s not the way the world works.

Andrew Watson:

If a magic wand exists, we haven’t found it in the teaching world yet.

Charles Fadel:

Or in the technology world.

Andrew Watson:

Thank you for taking the time to talk with me today.

Charles Fadel:

You’re welcome.

Charles Fadel Headshot

Charles Fadel is a global education thought leader and author, futurist and inventor; founder and chairman of the Center for Curriculum Redesign; visiting scholar at Harvard GSE; chair of the education committee at BIAC/OECD; co-author of “Four-Dimensional Education” (available in nine languages) and best-selling  “21st Century Skills”; founder and president of the Fondation Helvetica Educatio (Geneva, Switzerland); senior fellow at The Conference Board. He has worked with education systems and institutions in more than thirty countries. He was formerly Global Education Lead at Cisco Systems, visiting scholar at MIT ESG, and angel investor with Beacon Angels.  He holds a BSEE, an MBA, and seven patents.  Full Bio at:  http://curriculumredesign.org/about/team/#charles

 

Center for Curriculum Redesign

The mission of the Center for Curriculum Redesign (CCR) is to answer this timely question, and openly propagate its recommendations and frameworks on a worldwide basis. The CCR brings together non-governmental organizations, jurisdictions, academic institutions, corporations, and non-profit organizations including foundations.

The last major changes to curriculum were effected in the late 1800’s as a response to the sudden growth in societal and human capital needs. As the world of the 21st century bears little resemblance to that of the 19th century, education curricula need to be deeply redesigned for the four dimensions of Knowledge, Skills, Character and Meta-Learning.  Adapting to 21st century needs means revisiting each dimension and the interplay between them.

This paper outlines the CCR model for curriculum redesign.

4-Dimensional Education can be downloaded from the CCR website.

 

Benefiting from Retrieval Practice: Get the Timing Just Right
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Retrieval Practice Timing Affects its Benefits

I’ve posted a lot here recently about retrieval practice: the practice of reviewing material by pulling it OUT of the student’s head rather than trying to put it back IN.

For example: if I ask my students to write down the 5 main points from yesterday’s class about the Buddha, that’s retrieval practice. They have to get info out of their heads.

If, instead, I remind them of the 5 main points from yesterday’s class, that’s not retrieval practice. After all, I’m putting information back in.

The short sales pitch for retrieval practice is: it works for all students in all subjects, all the time. (Ask Dr. Pooja Agarwal.)

Unless…

Of course, all students in all subjects all the time is quite a grand claim. It’s rare for any teaching practice to work all the time, so we should be on the lookout for boundary conditions.

And, indeed, one has recently jumped out at me.

The story is interestingly complicated. I promise, however, that a close study of this complexity leads to specific and useful teaching advice. So: hang in there!

When Retrieval Practice Timing Might Be Bad

Imagine that, in yesterday’s class, we went over ten definitions for key economics terms. I want to begin today’s class with a quick review, so we go back over five of those terms.

My assumption is that, by reviewing five, I’m actually helping you to remember all ten.

Here’s the surprising research finding: by practicing some of the terms, I actually make it LESS LIKELY that you’ll remember the unpracticed terms.

In other words: recalling some of the words prompts you to forget the unpracticed words.

Psychologists call this bizarre result retrieval-induced forgetting. After all, the retrieval — that is, the practice — induced you to forget.

When Research Fields Contradict

So: the retrieval practice research says that retrieval is beneficial for memory.

And: the retrieval-induced forgetting research says that retrieval is detrimental for memory.

What happens when teachers do both? Does one cancel out the other? Can Superman defeat Iron Man?

Research done by Jason CK Chan helps answer this intriguing question.

The short answer is: in the short term, retrieval-induced forgetting is stronger. So: if I quiz you on five of those economics terms, and then give you the final test on those terms an hour later, you’re more likely to forget the five unpracticed words.

However, in the longer term, retrieval practice is stronger. So: the quiz on five terms will benefit you if you take that final test 24 hours later.

This result is especially likely if my quiz encourages you to think about how these five words connect conceptually to the other words.

Practical Advice

Although these research findings can be difficult to follow, they do all lead to a specific suggestion.

Retrieval practice is an excellent study strategy for students more than 24 hours ahead of a test. However, within that 24 hour window, teachers and students should focus more on connecting ideas rather than recalling them.

To update Dr. Agarwal’s guidance: retrieval practice works for all students in all subjects, (almost) all the time.