Skip to main content
Default Image
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Over at the Cult of Pedagogy, Jennifer Gonzalez has a FANTASTIC post summarizing lots of research on note-taking.

Some headlines:

Note-taking is a skill we should teach.

Visuals improve notes.

Pauses for revision and reflection help a lot.

I should note: Gonzalez cites the well-known Mueller & Oppenheimer study showing that handwritten notes help learning more than laptop notes do. Long-time readers know that I don’t think this study supports that conclusion.

In fact: I think it suggests that the opposite is true. My argument is here.

Despite our disagreement on this one point, there’s so much to savor in this summary that I recommend it highly.

Enjoy!

The Best Way to Read? Paper vs. Screens
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Do you have to print out emails before you read them, because you just hate screens?

paper vs. screens

Or, do you take your Kindle everywhere, because old-fashioned books are just too cumbersome?

The “paper vs. screens” debate has raged for quite a while now.

I occasionally visit schools that have “done away with books” altogether. For reasons of cost and convenience, administrators tell me, e-readers are the only way to go.

Paper vs. Screens: Today’s news

Although I have written about tentative answers to this question, we would love to inform the debate with substantive research. As of today, we can.

Lalo Salmeron and colleagues have completed a meta-analysis comparing the two formats. Their research included several dozen studies, and included more than 170,000 participants.

The results?

In almost every case, students understand better and learn more when they read from paper than when they read from screens.

Some highlights:

Surprisingly, we aren’t getting better at reading from screens. In fact, more recent screen technologies produce greater gaps than previously. As Salmeron writes, “the screen inferiority effect has increased in the past 18 years, and … there were no differences in media effects between age groups.”

Especially when students faced time pressure, the length of the text didn’t matter. That is: even short passages that don’t require scrolling are harder to understand on screen than on paper.

Another surprise: screens made reading information harder. But, they didn’t make reading narrative harder. The teaching implication: e-readers work better for novels than for textbooks.

Paper vs. Screens: Today’s reality

Salmeron’s team has a practical bent as well:

“Given the unavoidable inclusion of digital devices in our contemporary educational systems, more work must be done to train pupils … with reading tasks in digital media.”

On standardized tests, for instance, our students will almost certainly have to read on screens at important moments in their academic lives.

We do need more research on particular strategies. In the meanwhile, this article recommends Lauterman & Ackerman’s article on “Overcoming Screen Inferiority” for places to start.

In the meanwhile, we can help our students understand by having them read from good old-fashioned paper. And no: despite “cost and convenience,” e-readers are not the best way to learn.

Video: Stress and Memory
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

The folks over at TedEd have posted an excellent video exploring the relationship between stress and memory.

The video lasts only a few minutes, but it includes lots of helpful information.

stress and memeory

In particular, note that we can’t simply say “stress harms memory.” We always have to be more specific:

How much stress?

What kind of memory?

When does the stress take place?

What do we want our students to remember?

All these variables matter.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hyg7lcU4g8E

The Self-Control Paradox: Resistance is (Often) Futile
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Oscar Wilde famously said that he could resist everything but temptation.

This may be the only way that I’m a lot like Wilde. You, too, probably resemble this great Irish wit.

self-control paradox

Misunderstanding Self-Control

Self-control seems like a straightforward concept.

In front of me is a slice of chocolate cake. Or pizza. Or the very latest tech gizmo.

I really want it. Like, a lot.

But I steel myself and, like Odysseus resisting the Sirens, I deny myself this treat. That’s self-control.

Much of the research into self-control resembles this scenario. Roy Baumeister, one of the best-known self-control researchers, often asked study participants to resist chocolate chip cookies.

If you can resist freshly-baked chocolate chip cookies…well…that’s self-control.

Clearly, if we want to helps our students with self-control, we should help them resist such temptations.

The Self-Control Paradox

Or, maybe not.

Maybe you, and me, and Oscar Wilde are all really bad at resisting temptation. Maybe that’s not how self-control works at all.

Instead, in one of the most interesting psychology studies I’ve read, several researchers come to a different conclusion.

People who successfully resist temptation don’t stare down the chocolate chip cookies and boldly exert their self-control.

Instead, they use their self-control to avoid temptation in the first place.

That’s right: effective self-control isn’t exactly self-control. Instead, people who score highest on measures of self-control use it to develop virtuous habits and avoid tempting situations.

The reason I’m good at, say, staying on my diet is NOT that I resist cupcakes.

Instead, I’m good at dieting because a) I have gotten in the habit of making a salad, and b) my lunchtime walk never approaches the pizza joint.

I never have to resist temptation, because I use self-control to avoid temptation in the first place.

Odysseus, Reconsidered

Come to think of it: Odysseus didn’t use his self-control to resist the Sirens. He was, after all, tied to the mast at the time he sailed past them.

Instead, he used his self-control to prepare for temptation. Because he knew he couldn’t resist it, he made a plan to ensure the temptation wouldn’t lead him astray.

If we want our students to improve their executive functioning, if we want them to get better at self-control, then we should not focus on resisting temptation.

Instead, we should focus on avoiding temptation.

Resistance might be futile. But: the self-control paradox suggests we can bypass resistance altogether.

 

Twice Exceptional: Supporting and Educating Bright and Creative Students With Learning Difficulties by Scott Barry Kaufman
Rebecca Gotlieb
Rebecca Gotlieb

Students who have both exceptional talents and learning difficulties have been understudied and underserved in the educational system. Fortunately, Twice Exceptional: Supporting and Educating Bright and Creative Students with Learning Difficulties helps shed light on this unique and diverse population.  This volume contains contributions from over 30 talented scholars and practioners and was edited by Scott Barry Kaufman, who brings a special sensitivity to the topic because of his own experiences as a twice exceptional (2E) student. Kaufman, whose books we have reviewed previously (see here and here) envisions an educational system in which every student receives individualized, challenging, and supportive instruction. Until that dream is realized, it is especially important to provide that kind of individualization to 2E students or students who “demonstrate exceptional levels of capacity, competence, commitment, or creativity in one or more domains coupled with one or more learning difficulties” (P. 7). This book, filled with examples of students with varying 2E profiles, provides guidance about best practices for identifying and supporting 2E students, spotlights specific 2E sub-populations, and concludes with examples of schools that successfully support 2E students.

Although there is research that recognized 2E students dating back to the 1920s, federal education laws have been slow to do so.  In the 1980s there was a shift in gifted education towards recognizing a greater variety of gifts and trying to develop students’ talents rather than assuming that students are either gifted or not. It was not until 2004 that federal disability laws first acknowledged that students with disabilities can also be gifted. Today, parents still face resistance in getting services for 2E children. For example, while 7% of the general student population participates in gifted education, only 1% of students who receive disability services are involved in gifted education.

Contributing to this problem may be difficulties related to defining and assessing both “disability” and “giftedness.’ Additionally, the interacting effects of students’ disabilities and giftedness can contribute to the students’ unique profiles not being recognized. That is one’s exceptionality can hide his disabilities (making adults characterize the student as lazy), one’s disabilities can hide her exceptionalities (making adults underestimate the student’s potential), or one’s disabilities and exceptionalities can each mask the other (making adults miss great potential and great need for support). What 2E students need is to have their unique intersections of exceptionality carefully described and monitored across time and context. Several chapters in this collection provide guidelines for comprehensively assessing students’ learning profiles.

2E students need their strengths, talents, and interests developed.  Unfortunately, too much attention is given to compensating for these student’s weaknesses rather than developing their strengths.  We should strive to help them reach their fullest potential.  We should support students in being assertive self-advocates. When they can advocate for themselves they typically set higher expectations for themselves than others would set for them. Beyond self-advocacy, tending to and developing the social-emotional needs and abilities of 2E students is important for helping them have the resilience to persist through the difficulties they will inevitably face in school. Another key principle for supporting 2E students is creating effective collaboration among family members, teachers, friends, coaches, psychologists, and medical professionals. This book offers several strategies for strengthening the relationship between families and schools.

Several chapters in Twice Exceptional profile certain categories of 2E populations and offer strategies for supporting these populations with a strengths-based approach.  For example, attention deficit hyperactive disorder could be reframed as an attention divergent hyperactive gift.  Similar ideas are applied to students with autism spectrum disorders, sensory processing issues, and dyslexia or other reading disorders.

One of the most valuable contributions of this book is the focus in two chapters, on an especially underserved 2E sub-population—that of 2E students who are also racial or ethnic minorities. For several problematic reasons, students of color are underrepresented in gifted education. Having other exceptionalities compounds this problem further. To serve 2E students of color it is important to remain flexible, set high expectations, offer role models, and be creative about how to draw out these students’ strengths.

Twice Exceptional concludes by describing real model schools that are designed to support 2E students.  These schools celebrate strengths, differentiate curriculum, incorporate social-emotional learning into academic learning, provide a safe space filled with positive peer and teacher relationship, and remain flexible, patient, and optimistic.  Above all, they develop autonomy in their students by encouraging the 2E students to assume primary responsibility for shaping their learning.

Kaufman’s Twice Exceptionaloffers a much need consolidation of information about 2E students to raise awareness about this population and help researchers, parents, and educators better serve 2E students.

Kaufman, S. B. (Ed.). (2018). Twice Exceptional: Supporting and Educating Bright and Creative Students with Learning Difficulties. Oxford University Press.

Do We Actually Know What We Think We Know?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Teachers worry a lot about stress. For that reason, this blog regularly scans research updates for useful news about stress and stress reduction techniques.

replication crisis

(In fact, I posted about potential benefits of stress just two weeks ago.)

One of our favorite studies offers a surprisingly simple approach to stress reduction: writing about it.

We like this study so much, we’ve posted about it twice: here and here.

So here’s an important question: do we actually know that the strategy works?

Put in a different way: how do psychologists and neuroscientists know what they claim to know?

Replication, Replication Crisis

To answer this question, we can describe research methodology. The ritual of science publication, in fact, requires almost gruesomely soporific descriptions of EXACTLY what the scientists did.

(What computer model did they use? What software version did they use? What size was the font? How far away from the computer did the participants sit? You get the idea…)

Here’s another answer: to be sure we know what we know, we double check. When one group of researchers arrives at a conclusion — especially an exciting or novel conclusion — other researchers see if they get the same result.

To be fancy, we can say they try to “replicate” the original result. As we get more replications, we feel more confident that we know.

Here’s the problem: psychology is facing a replication crisis.

A Case in Point: Writing About Stress

In recent years, more and more psychology studies just don’t replicate. We thought we knew, but now we’re not so sure.

For example: that study we love? The one about “reducing stress by writing about it?” A new replication crisis project tried to replicate it, and got no results.

In this replication, the benefits of writing about stress were…nada.

In fact, this group of researchers tried to replicate all 21 studies published in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Depending on how you count, 13 of them replicated. That means that eight of them did not replicate.

YIKES.

What’s a Teacher to Do?

This news might seem like a disaster. If so much research doesn’t replicate, we might be tempted to stop relying on science in our teaching.

Although understandable, this dis-spirited conclusion goes too far. Instead, the replication crisis should remind us of two essential points:

First: teachers should adopt teaching ideas that have lots of research support.

If you see one study suggesting that (say) chewing gum helps students convert fractions to decimals, DO NOT adopt that technique. At a minimum, you should look to see if other researchers have replicated that finding.

In fact, you’d like several researchers to have explored this idea — preferably in different grades and schools, with different research paradigms.

(You should also weigh the other pluses and minuses. All that gum chewing might be really distracting.)

Second: we should be ready to change our minds. If a wave of research points one direction, we might adopt a particular strategy.

However, if better research over longer periods of time discounts those findings, then we have to be willing to try something else.

The best-known example of this problem: “learning styles.” Early research suggested that they might exist, but the vast weight of evidence shows clearly that they don’t.

In brief: we should adopt evidence-based theories only if we’re willing to let go of them as further evidence dictates.

Final Thoughts

But what about that stress-reduction technique? Do we have to give it up, now that it didn’t replicate?

That’s a surprisingly complicated question.

True enough: this attempt at replication failed.

However, in the original study, researchers Ramirez and Beilock tried their technique twice — just to be sure they had it right.

In fact, they tried it twice in the psychology lab and twice more in a local high school.

And, a similar research paradigm arrived at similar results.

We can say that the study has an uneven replication record. That’s not the same thing as “didn’t replicate.”

For these reasons, I think we should put the “writing about stress” strategy in the “we just don’t know” category. It might work. It might not.

It might depend on circumstances that our research paradigms haven’t yet revealed.

When psychology research offers you contradictory advice, think about your own classroom circumstances and make the best decision you can.

That advice is complicated, but it’s certainly not a crisis.

 

Sad News
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

 

Like so many who study psychology, we at LatB are terribly sad to learn that Walter Mischel has died.

The New York Times obituary describes his importance — both in revolutionizing the field of psychology, and in popular understanding of self-control.

Personality in Context

For psychologists, Mischel emphasized the importance of context.

Personality theory suggests that “I’m this kind of person, not that kind of person.” Mischel’s research emphasizes that “I’m this kind of person in these circumstances, and that kind of person in other circumstances.” Context always matters.

This insight can’t be over-emphasized among teachers who want to teach with research in mind.

We shouldn’t just hear about a particular psychology study and adopt its methodology. We should, instead, check to see if the method that worked in that context might also work in this context. Which is to say: our classroom.

After all: most psychology research happens with college students.  If you don’t teach college students, you need to adapt that research to your context.

Marshmallows in Context

In popular culture, Mischel is best known for the “marshmallow test” (which, by the way, included other treats beyond marshmallows”).

You’ve seen videos of adorable 5-year-olds desperately trying to resist yummy goodness. For example:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4L-n8Z7G0ic

The often-oversimplified test includes many nuances. For instance, the 5-year-olds respond differently based on how much they trust you.

But the headline remains important: self-control matters.

Those of us who got to see Walter Mischel speak at the November 2015 conference will not soon forget his clarity, thoughtfulness, and warmth.

Have We Finally Arrived at 2nd Grade?
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

When I first met him, Kurt Fischer used to say “when it comes to the brain, we’re all still in kindergarten.”

(Who’s Kurt Fischer? Well, if you want to connect psychology, neuroscience, and education, you’re following Kurt’s work.

He started the first journal in the field, and the first graduate program. He founded the International Mind Brain Education Society.

He even helped Kelly Williams organize the very first Learning and the Brain conference.)

new brain cell

Dr. Fischer meant that even though we’ve been studying the brain for over 100 years, we still haven’t figured out very much about this infinitely complex part of our daily lives.

Years later, by the time I finished graduate school, he had started to admit we might be in Brain 1st Grade.

Exciting brain scanning techniques — especially fMRI — have shown us extraordinary and unexpected truths about our brains’ development and function.

Brain 2nd Grade?

Two recent discoveries make me wonder: are we in second grade yet?

First, a just-published study identifies a new brain cell — one that might, in fact, be unique to humans.

We don’t know much about “rosehip neurons”; for instance, we don’t know exactly what they do. They’re a kind of inhibitory neuron, but what they inhibit and why, we don’t yet know.

More than 100 years after Santiago Ramon y Cajal produced his amazing drawings of various brain cell types, we still haven’t identified them all.

Second, another just-published study suggests a new way for the brain to communicate with the body’s immune system.

The details here are quite complex (unless you’re already up to speed on aseptic meningitis and neutrophils).

But the surprise remains. Even now, we’re still figuring out basics: like, how does the brain talk with the immune system?

New Brain Cell: Classroom Implications

Of course, this news doesn’t yet tell us how to teach differently.

This new brain cell does, however, provide us an important reminder. Practically everything we learn about the brain in the 20-teens and 2020s will be tentative, initial, and incomplete. We should be excited with each development…and always ready to have old beliefs overturned by new findings.

Update on “Collaborative Learning”
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Last week, I wrote about a potential strategy for making group-work more effective. A Boston-based research team has found reason to think that “intermittent” collaboration might yield better results than constant (or absent) collaboration.collaborative learning

Although I’m excited to see these results, my article concluded with two concerns:

First: the research was done with college students. It might not apply to younger learners.

Second: the participants weren’t exactly collaborating. They were (or were not) looking at each others’ answers after they solved problems.

They did not do what typically happens in schools, where students work on problems and projects together all at the same time.

So, again: this research might not (or, might) apply in our classrooms.

(I should be clear: the researchers don’t claim to be studying collaboration. Their research field is “collective intelligence.” The most obvious place to apply their research is in what teachers call “collaboration.”)

Thoughtful Update for Concern #1…

When I have questions about a study, I try to ask the researchers for their thoughts. In this case, I reached out to Dr. Jesse Shore with my two concerns.

His answers struck me as particularly helpful, and so I’m sharing them with you. (I’m rewording passages from his email, but with his permission drawing substantially on them.)

First, Dr. Shore explains why intermittent “collaboration” helps:

The results depend on (1) people trying more diverse solutions when they are not seeing others’ solutions [ACW: that’s how working alone helps]

and (2) learning from the solutions of others when they do see them [ACW: that’s how “collaborating” helps].

Intermittent collaboration makes time for both benefits.

For this reason, Dr. Shore suspects that this strategy would work well with younger students. There’s no obvious reason why 3rd graders (for example) wouldn’t “try more diverse solutions when not seeing others’ solutions,” nor why they wouldn’t “learn from others’ solutions when they see them.”

Such hypotheses need testing, but that’s a plausible set of presumptions with which to start.

…and Concern #2

Dr. Shore shares my concern about applying “collective intelligence” research directly to “collaboration.” After all, collaboration includes at least two other key variables.

The “free rider” problem. When groups work together, some people can just sit back and let others do all the work.

Interpersonal relationships. In groups, some people like each other and work well together; others just don’t. Or, a student might adopt another student’s strategy not because it works well, but because that student is popular.

Despite these other variables, Dr. Shore writes, “my guess is that intermittent interaction would still be best.”

After all–as I think about his summary–it seems clear that intermittent collaboration would interrupt the “free-rider” problem. I can’t let you do all the work if I have to work by myself at times.

The Big Picture

First: Dr. Shore offers us good reasons to think that “intermittent” interaction would indeed benefit typical kinds of school collaboration.

Its benefits, most likely, help students of all ages. And it might help with (and certainly wouldn’t exacerbate) the additional complexities of full-on collaboration.

Second: this strikes me as an excellent example of the philosophy that teachers shouldn’t just “do this thing,” but instead should “think this way.”

In this case: when we see research about “collaboration,” we should not simply enact its guidance. Instead, we should contemplate the specific ways it does, and does not, fit exactly with what we do.

The Unexpected Dangers of Reading (and Writing) Blogs
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

A recent post on a well-known education blog beats up on that old nemesis: “rote memorization.”

To highlight this point, the author links to a study on the benefits of “the generation effect.”

When students try to guess at answers to questions, they’re likelier to remember the correct answer even if their initial guess was wrong.

Here’s the study.

Presumably, the study compares “the generation effect” to “rote memorization.” Presumably it concludes the former helps more than the latter.

Nope. Not a bit of it.

This study, instead, looks at different kinds of mistakes that students might make when they guess. It concludes that nearby guesses help, but far away guesses don’t.

Here’s the researchers’ summary: “errors benefit memory to the extent that they overlap semantically with targets.”

The study just isn’t about rote memorization. (Just to be sure, I checked with the study’s lead author. She confirms my understanding of the research.)

Lesson Learned

If you can’t even trust [name-of-well-known-education-blog], what can you do? How can you trust any news from the interwebs.

Here’s my advice:

First: anyone who quotes research should link to it. If you can’t find the link quite easily, don’t make any changes to your teaching.

Second: click the link. (Don’t yet make any changes to your teaching.)

Third: feel a little nervous. You’ve got several daunting pages of big words and bizarro graphs.

Fourth: Think about giving in and just making the changes that the article suggests. Don’t give in to that impulse. No changes yet.

Fifth: read the first paragraph of the research. It’s called the “abstract,” and it should summarize what the researchers did and what they concluded.

If the abstract DOESN’T include the point you read on the website, then you’re done. The research doesn’t focus on the argument that the blogger is making, and so shouldn’t have been cited in the first place.

Lesson learned. Nothing to see here. Move on.

In this case: when you read the abstract about the generation effect, you’d see that it never mentions rote memorization.

Of course, if the abstract DOES include that point — now you can start taking the blog seriously. You might not decide to change your teaching, but at least you’ve got an argument worth considering.

Flipping the Script

I had a similar experience (in reverse) about a month ago. This blog’s software notified me that another blog had linked to my article on omega-3 fish oil.

That post isn’t obviously the sexiest one on the website, so I was a bit surprised to see it getting internet love.

A brief investigation showed that the link came from a website in a foreign language — one that encouraged pregnant women to buy omega-3 fish oil supplements.

Hmmm.

My article summarized research showing that such supplements don’t help with working memory or in-school behavior. Not so much about benefits for pregnant women.

It seems clear that this other blog assumed its readers a) wouldn’t click on the link, and b) if they did, they wouldn’t be able to read English well.

The link was there to fool their readers, not help them.

The Headline

When we see a blog link to research, we can feel reassured. At the other end of the link, doubtless, lies research supporting the author’s point.

Don’t believe it. Don’t trust. Verify.

Click the link. Read the abstract…