I’m on my annual vacation during this month, so I’ll be posting some articles that got attention during the last year.
This post, initially from December of 2021, looks at a proposed different way to “put all the research pieces together.”
You want to improve your teaching with psychology research?
We’ve got good news, and bad news.
And more good news.
Good News: we have lots and LOTS of research. We can talk about attention, or working memory, or the spacing effect, or motivation, or stress…the list is long. And super helpful.
So much practical advice!
Bad News: actually, the bad news is the same as the good news. We’ve got SO MUCH good research that it’s honestly hard to keep track of it all.
I mean, seriously. Should you start by looking at attention research? Or stress research?
Should we think about the motivational effects of student-teacher relationships, or the perils of working memory overload, or the benefits of desirable difficulty?
Which is most important?
Honestly, I think our next priority is not so much finding out new truths about learning, but organizing all the information we already have.
More Good News
If you agree that we really need someone to sort all these suggestions into a coherent system, you’ll be delighted to read this article by Stephen Chew (Twitter handle: @SChewPsych) and William Cerbin (@BillCerbin).
Other scholars — for instance, Barak Rosenshine — have put together a coherent system based on learning principles. Chew and Cerbin, instead, organize their system around cognitive challenges.
That is:
If students feel anxiety about a topic or discipline, that emotion will interfere with their learning.
If students have prior misconceptions, they will distort students’ understanding.
If classroom work or assignments go beyond working memory limits, students won’t learn effectively (or, at all).
When planning a course or a lesson or an assignment, teachers can think their way through these specific challenges. By contemplating each one, we can design our work to best facilitate learning.
Getting the Emphasis Right
If you’re thinking “this is such excellent news! It just can’t get any better!” — well — I’ve got some news: it gets better.
Chew and Cerbin write:
There is no single best teaching strategy for all students, topics, and situations. The proposed framework is not prescriptive … and can guide adaptation of teaching practice.
In other words, they’re not saying: here’s a list of things to do.
Instead, they are saying: here are several topics/problems to consider.
Teaching advice should not include “best practices.” (That’s a business concept.) It should include “best questions to ponder as we make decisions.” Chew and Cerbin make this point repeatedly.
Frequent readers know that I’ve been banging on for years with this mantra: “Don’t just do this thing; instead, think this way.”
We should think about our students’ working memory limitations. The strategies we use might differ for 1st graders and 8th graders.
We should think about the importance of transfer. A Montessori school and a KIPP school will (almost certainly) use differing strategies to reach that goal.
We should think about our students’ prior knowledge. The best way to measure that knowledge might be different for students with diagnosed learning differences.
Yes: we should consider these nine topics. But the ways we answer them must depend on our students, our schools, our curriculum, and ourselves.
For all these reasons, I recommend Chew and Cerbin’s article with great enthusiasm.
And, happily, you can meet Dr. Chew at our online conference in February! (In case you’re wondering: I was planning to write about this article before I knew he was joining the conference. A happy synchronicity.)
Some research-based teaching advice requires complex rethinking of our work.
For instance:
We know that “desirable difficulties” like spacing and interleaving help students learn. At the same time, this strategy might require a fair amount of reorganization in our unit plans.
On the other hand, today’s suggestions could hardly be simpler. They go like this:
First: encourage students to use a straightforward self-talk strategy (see below).
Second: remind them frequently.
The likely result; they’ll learn more!
The First Step: A Self-Talk Strategy
Our students often know what they should do. But, they struggle to make themselves do it.
So many plausible excuses. So many reasons NOT to follow through!
Over two decades ago, researchers devised a remarkably simple way to redirect those excuses: “if-then” statements.
The strategy goes like this:
The student starts by setting a goal.
“I want to read 15 pages of the novel.”
She then lists the likely problems that could interfere with that goal.
“My dog could distract me.”
“I could get bored.”
“I could get a text.”
Next, she lists solutions to those likely problems — phrased as “if-then” statements.
“If my dog distracts me, then I’ll ask my brother to play with him.”
“If my phone buzzes, then I’ll turn it off and give it to my parents.”
We have good research suggesting that this “if-then” structure creates highly beneficial mental shortcuts.
At the moment of distraction, the student doesn’t have to decide what to do. She has already decided; she simply has to execute the plan she made for herself.
This strategy sounds too simple to work. But, we’ve got good research suggesting it does.
A Second Step: Well-timed Reminders
So far, this strategy could hardly be simpler. Students set goals, make “if-then” plans, and get to work.
A recent study asks a useful question: how often should teachers remind students about these “if-then” plans?
Here’s a useful analogy:
When you take a medication, you probably don’t take all of it all at once. Instead, you probably take one pill a day — or something like that. The whole course of medication is divided into doses.
Well, should teachers divide this strategy into doses? Does it matter how often we prescribe this study “medication”?
Researchers, led by Dr. Jasmin Breitwieser, explored these questions with medical students in Germany.
The details of the study get complicated quickly. But the headlines go like this:
Students in the control group set daily study goals, but did not make if-then statements.
Students in the study group also set daily goals, and “internalized” this statement:
If I am thinking about stopping [before I reach my goal], then I will tell myself that I will continue to answer questions until I have reached my intended workload!
Students in this second group got reminders on various schedules — as many as 3 days in a row, and as many as 3 days without a reminder.
So: what did the researchers find?
Results Please
First: Breitwieser’s team found that students who made the if-then commitments scored higher on the exam than those who didn’t. *
Second — an this is the big news: dosing mattered.
When students got several reminders in a row, they reached more of their goals.
When those same students got no reminders for 3 days in a row, they reached less of their goals.
In other words: if-then statements help students achieve — especially if they recommit to them frequently.
Teaching Implications
Teachers really struggle to help students find motivation in their school work.
This if-then strategy provides a remarkably simple way to help students achieve their own goals.
So, suggestion #1: teachers should take time to help students formulate these if-then statements.
Suggestion #2: one dose isn’t enough. We should have students return to these plans frequently.
The exact schedule will depend, I suspect, on your specific circumstances. It will be different for 1st graders, 5th graders, and 9th graders. It will be different depending on your school and your culture, and perhaps even your approach to teaching.
Based on other research pools, I suggest that the reminders be relatively frequent, but unpredictable. We don’t want our students going through this process by rote; they should engage with it as earnestly as possible each time.
If we get the dosing right, this simple strategy can help our students study more effectively — and therefore learn more.
* The difference was quite small: only 2 points. However, and this is a big however, the researchers worked with medical students preparing for a high-stakes exam. They are already highly successful and highly motivated students. I suspect we’ll see more dramatic effects for other groups of students.
Breitwieser, J., Neubauer, A. B., Schmiedek, F., & Brod, G. (2021). Self-regulation prompts promote the achievement of learning goals–but only briefly: Uncovering hidden dynamics in the effects of a psychological intervention. Learning and Instruction, 101560.
Imagine that you work at a school where these students consistently struggle compared to those students.
As teachers and school leaders, you’d like to help these students do better than they currently do; maybe do as well as those students. (Lower down in the post, I’ll say more about the two groups of students.)
What can you do?
Values Affirmation
One simple strategy has gotten a fair amount of research in recent years.
The idea goes something like this. If I am, say, a 12-year-old student, I might not really see how my school life fits with the rest of my life. They seem like two different world.
If teachers could connect those “two different worlds” — even a little bit — students would feel more comfortable, less stressed, better invested. Heck, they might even learn more.
To accomplish this mission, researchers provide students a list of values: loyalty, faith, friendship, hard work, justice, happiness, family, and so forth.
They then have students undertake a brief writing assignment. The instructions go something like this:
Choose one or more of these values that are most important for you personally. Write about why they are important. You won’t be graded on spelling or grammar; focus on explaining your ideas, values, and beliefs clearly.
The control group gets the same instructions, except that students choose values “that are the least important for you personally, but might be important to someone else.”
This strategy gets the somewhat lumpy name “values affirmation” — because it gives students a chance to affirm the values they hold.
This approach has been used in the United States in several studies, and has had some success. (See, for instance, here).
Across the Atlantic
But: would this strategy work elsewhere? What about, say, England?
They had two groups of 11-14-year-old students (the underperformers, the typical performers) do three “values affirmation” writing sessions: one in September, one in January, and one in April.
Of course, one half of both groups did the “values affirmation” version of the writing exercises; the other half did the control writing assignment.
What results did they find?
As was true in the US, the values affirmation writings had no effect on the typical performers.
However, it had a dramatic effect on the underperformers. Their math grades were higher at the end of the year (compared to the group that did the control writing). And their stress levels were considerably lower.
Because of the statistical method that the researchers used, I can’t say that values affirmation translated a B average into an A average. I can say that it had an effect size of 0.35 standard deviations — which is certainly noteworthy, especially to people who read research studies in this field.
In brief: this strategy costs literally zero dollars. It takes one hour over the course of a school year. And it helps underperforming students.
SO MUCH TO LOVE.
The Story Behind the Story
Up to this point, I’ve described what the researchers did. But I haven’t explained the psychological theory behind their strategy — because the theory prompts some controversy.
Now that we’ve looked at the strategy, let’s get to that theory.
Back in 1995, Claude Steele proposed a theory that has come to be called “Stereotype Threat.” It proposes a complex and counter-intuitive hypothesis.
To describe the theory, let me make up a non-existent stereotype: “blue-eyed people are bad at grammar.” (For the record, I’m regularly complimented on my blue eyes, and I teach a lot of grammar.)
Why might a blue-eyed student struggle on a grammar test?
Steele’s research suggests a surprising internal process. When my blue eyes and I sit down to take the grammar test, I know the material well. However, I also know that stereotypes suggest I’ll do badly.
What happens? I do NOT (as many suspect) give in and let the stereotype become a self-fulfilling prophecy. Instead, I decide to fight back. And, in a terrible paradox, my determination to disprove the stereotype leads to all sorts of counter-productive academic behaviors.
For instance: I might spend lots of time working on a very easy problem to prove that I know this grammar. Alas, I took so long on the easy problems that I don’t have time for the harder ones.
In this unexpected way, Steele argues, stereotypes harm students’ learning.
Enter the Controversy
Over the years, many researchers explored Stereotype Threat. They found that stereotypes about almost anything (race, ethnicity, gender, sexuality, academic major) can affect performance on almost anything (math tests, sports performance, leadership aspirations).
Steele’s book Whistling Vivaldi is, in fact, an unusually easy-to-read book about a complex psychological phenomenon. Many Learning and the Brain speakers (Joshua Aronson, Sian Beilock) have studied and written about ST.
At the same time, other scholars have doubted this entire research field. They point to various statistical and procedural concerns to suggest that, well, there’s no real there there. (If you’re interested in the push back, you can read more here and here.)
Putting It All Together
In the English study I’ve been describing, the relevant stereotype is “people from relatively lower socio-economic status just aren’t as smart as others.” According to the study’s authors, this stereotype is the predominant academic stereotype in England, whereas US stereotypes focus more on race, ethnicity, and gender.
So, in their study, the authors explored the effect of Values Affirmation on students who did (and did not) receive free school lunches: a common proxy for socio-economic status.
Sure enough, Values Affirmation had no effect one way or the other on students who did not receive free lunches. Because they faced fewer stereotypes about their academic performance, they didn’t suffer the harm that ST might cause.
But, for the students who DID receive free lunches, that same writing exercise helped a lot. This strategy made them feel more like they belonged, so they presumably didn’t need to work as hard to disprove stereotypes.
For that reason, as described above, one hour’s worth of writing reduced stressed and increased grades.
TL;DR
This research suggests that a Values Affirmation writing assignment (it’s free!) can help some underperforming students learn more and feel less stress.
And, it also strengthens the case that Stereotype Threat might — despite the concerns about methodology — really be a thing.
Even if that second statement turns out not to be true, the first one is worth highlighting.
Want a simple, low-cost way to help struggling students? We’ve got one…
Hadden, I. R., Easterbrook, M. J., Nieuwenhuis, M., Fox, K. J., & Dolan, P. (2020). Self‐affirmation reduces the socioeconomic attainment gap in schools in England. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 517-536.
Sherman, D. K., Hartson, K. A., Binning, K. R., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Taborsky-Barba, S., … & Cohen, G. L. (2013). Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: how self-affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 591.
When I talk with teachers about psychology and neuroscience research, I frequently get a question in this shape:
“I’ve heard that X is really good for the brain. Is that really true?”
In this sentence, X might be blueberries. It might be water. It might be nature walks. Perhaps it’s a good night’s sleep, or green tea, or coffee, or merlot ice cream. (I think I made up that last one, but anything’s possible…)
So, should schools start serving blueberries, merlot ice cream, and green tea (and black coffee) to our students? Perhaps with a side of salmon — brain food for sure!
Works (Almost) Every Time
Here is a completely unsurprising research finding: the brain is a part of the body.
The brain is, in fact, physically attached to the body.
For this reason, everything that is good for the body is good for the brain. (Because, again, the brain is a part of the body.)
Is sleep good for the brain? Well, it’s good for the body, so; yes.
How about water? Yup.
Fruits/veggies? Sure.
Exercise? I’m in!
Simply put, when we take care good care of our bodies, we simultaneously tend to our brains — as a physical, biological object.
Said the other way around: we don’t need to develop special “brain enhancing” diets or programs or regimens. Anything that promotes our students’ physical health will automatically help their brains.
I was, in fact, inspired to write this post by an article I saw today about pollution. The summary:
“Higher exposure to air pollution is associated with higher functional brain connectivity among several brain regions in preadolescents.”
This conclusion strikes me as entirely sensible. Pollution changes the body; unsurprisingly it changes the brain. (Say it with me: the brain is a part of the body.)
Checking the Details
This first answer to the question works most of the time.
If, however, we need a more specific answer, we can easily investigate.
I once heard that, because brains need appropriate levels of hydration, we should think of water as “brain food.” The speaker exhorted us with this cry: “A bottle of water on every desk!”
And yet, the speaker’s logic collapses immediately. Yes, too little water is bad for the brain (because it’s bad for the body). We do want students to be properly hydrated.
But this obvious truth does not remotely suggest that additional water above that level yields extra benefits.
Yes, we should let students drink if they’re thirsty. Yes, a hot day in an arid climate might prompt us to provide “a glass of water on every desk.”
But we don’t need to make a big deal about extra water as an avenue toward extra learning.
You won’t be surprised to know: when I googled “Water is brain food,” the top hits were NOT research studies. They were advertisements for companies selling water.
Magical Blueberries
For reasons I don’t fully understand, the “brain food” claim often settles on blueberries. They’ve got antioxidants, I’m told. They’re great.
I’ve done just a little research here, and so far I’m underwhelmed.
First: there honestly isn’t much research on this topic.
Second: the research often focuses on rats. (Long time readers know my mantra: “Never change your teaching based on research into non-human animals.)
Third: the research on humans focuses on aging and dementia.
Now, I’m 56. I’m ALL IN FAVOR of dietary changes that reduce the likelihood of dementia.
But the idea that “because blueberries are brain food, students should nosh on them before a test” has absolutely no research backing (that I can find).
Students should eat blueberries because fruits and vegetable — in the right proportion — provide health benefits for the body. As far as I can tell, we don’t need to focus on targeted brain benefits.
TL;DR
Most everything that is good for the body is also good for the brain. So, don’t worry about special “brain benefit” claims.
If, instead, someone claims that X is good for learning, we teachers should indeed pay close attention — and especially pay attention to the details of the research.
When teachers get advice from psychology and neuroscience, we start by getting individual bits of guidance. For instance…
… mindful meditation reduces stress, or
… growth mindset strategies (done the right way) can produce modest benefits, or
… cell phones both distract students and reduce working memory.
Each single suggestion has its uses. We can weave them, one at a time, into our teaching practices.
After a while, we start asking broader questions: how can we best combine all those individual advice bits?
For instance: might the benefits of growth mindset strategies offset the detriments of cell phones?
Happily, in recent years, researchers have started to explore these combination questions.
Retrieval Practice, Generative Learning
Long time readers know about the benefits of retrieval practice. Rather than simply review material, students benefit when they actively try to recall it first.
So too, generative learning strategies have lots of good research behind them. When students have to select, organize, and integrate information on their own, this mental exercise leads to greater learning. (Check out a handy book review here.)
Now that we have those individual bits of guidance, can we put them together? What’s the best way to combine retrieval practice with generative learning?
Researchers in Germany had college students study definitions of 8 terms in the field of “social attribution.”
So, for instance, they studied one-sentence definitions of “social norms” or “distinctiveness” or “self-serving bias.”
One group — the control group — simply studied these definitions twice.
A second group FIRST reviewed these words with retrieval practice, and THEN generated examples for these concepts (that’s generative learning).
A third group FIRST generated examples, and THEN used retrieval practice.
So, how well did these students remember the concepts — 5 minutes later, or one day later?
The Envelope Please
The researchers wanted to know: does the order (retrieval first? generation first?) matter?
The title of their study says it all: “Sequence Matters! Retrieval practice before generative learning is more effective than the reverse order.”
Both 5 minutes later and the next day, students who did retrieval practice first remembered more than those who came up with examples first (and, more than the control group).
For a variety of statistical reasons, I can’t describe how much better they did. That is: I can’t say “These student scored a B, and these score a B-.” But, they did “better enough” for statistical models to notice the difference.
And so, very tentatively, I think we teachers can plan lessons in this way: first instruct, then have students practice with retrieval, then have them practice with generation.
Wait, Why “Tentatively”?
If the research shows that “retrieval first” helps students more than “generation first,” why am I being tentative?
Here’s why:
We can’t yet say that “research shows” the benefits of a retrieval-first strategy.
Instead, we can say that this one study with these German college students who learned definitions of words suggests that conclusion.
But: we need many more studies of this question before we can spot a clear pattern.
And: we’d like some 1st grade students in Los Angeles, and some 8th grade students in Reykjavik, and some adult learners in Cairo before we start thinking of this conclusion as broadly applicable.
And: we’d like to see different kinds of retrieval practice, and different kinds of generative learning strategies, before we reach a firm conclusion.
After all, Garvin Brod has found that different generative learning strategies have different levels of effectiveness in various grades. (Check out this table from this study.)
To me, it seems entirely plausible that students need to retrieve ideas fluently before they can generate new ideas with them: hence, retrieval practice before generative learning.
But, “entirely plausible” isn’t a research-based justification. It’s a gut feeling. (In fact, for various reasons, the researchers had predicted the opposite finding.)
So, I think teachers should know about this study, and should include it our thinking.
But, we shouldn’t think it’s an absolute conclusion. If our own students simply don’t learn well with this combination, we might think about switching up the order.
TL;DR
Students learn more from retrieval practice, and they learn more from generative learning strategies.
If we want to combine those individual strategies, we’ll (probably) help students more if we start with retrieval practice.
And: we should keep an eye out for future research that confirms — or complicates — this advice.
Roelle, J., Froese, L., Krebs, R., Obergassel, N., & Waldeyer, J. (2022). Sequence matters! Retrieval practice before generative learning is more effective than the reverse order. Learning and Instruction, 80, 101634.
Brod, G. (2020). Generative learning: Which strategies for what age?. Educational Psychology Review, 1-24.
In the third Die Hard movie, Brue Willis and his unexpected partner Samuel L. Jackson need to get to Wall Street a hurry. They commandeer a cab.
An experienced cab driver, Jackson suggests taking 9th Avenue south, but Willis insists on going through Central Park.
It turns out: he doesn’t mean taking the road that runs through the Central Park, but driving through the park itself — across crowded lawns, through busy playgrounds, past famous fountains, down winding bike-paths.
His desperate short-cut helps the team catch up.
In education these days, it seems that we need our very own Bruce Willis.
Because of Covid, our students are WAY BEHIND.
5th graders don’t know as much math as they used to. 2nd graders can’t read as well as they once could. 9th graders have lost even more social skills than 9th graders usually lose.
Because our students know less and can do less, we teachers want to help them CATCH UP.
And so we ask: what’s the educational analogue to driving through the park? How can we — like Bruce and Samuel — help our students learn faster?
Like lots of folks, I’ve been thinking about that question for a while now. I’ve got bad news, and worse news; and I’ve got good news.
The Bad News
The Bruce Willis Method does not exist in education.
We can’t “drive through the park.” We can’t, in other words, help students “learn the same amount, only faster.”
Here’s why I say so:
If we knew how to teach any faster, we would have been doing so already.
Seriously. Do you know any teacher who says, “I could have covered this curriculum in 10 weeks. But what the heck, I’m going to drag it out and take 12 or 13”?
I don’t. And I suspect you don’t either.
We have always been helping our students learn as best we could. If we knew better ways, we would have been using them.
Of course Willis can get through the park faster; it was a MOVIE! Alas, we can’t follow his example.
I am, in fact, quite worried about all the talk of “catching up.” In my mind, it creates two clear dangers:
First Danger:
If we try to catch up, we’ll probably — in one way or another — try to speed up. We will, for instance, explore a topic in 2 weeks instead of 3 weeks. We will combine 3 units into 1.
However, the decision to speed up necessarily means that students spend less time thinking about a particular topic.
As Dan Willingham has taught us: “memory is the residue of thought.” If students spend less time thinking about a topic, they will learn less about it.
The result: they won’t catch up. Instead, they will be further behind.
In other words: such efforts to help students recover from Covid learning muddle will — paradoxically — hinder their learning.
Second Danger:
If we believe that “catching up” is a realistic short-term possibility, we open ourselves up to inspiring-but-unfounded claims.
People who don’t work in schools will tell us that “you can’t solve problems with the same thinking that created those problems in the first place.”
Their claims might include words & phrases like “transformational” or “thinking outside the box” or “new paradigm” or “disrupt.”
These claims will almost certainly come with products to buy: new technology here, new textbooks there, new mantras yon.
They will sound uplifting and exciting and tempting and plausible.
But…
… any “research-based” claims will almost certainly extrapolate substantially beyond the research’s actual findings;
… these ideas won’t have been tested at scale in a realistic setting;
… such claims will defy core knowledge about cognitive architecture. (No, students can’t overcome working memory limitations simply because “they can look up everything on the internet.”)
In other words: because the goal (“catching up”) is so tempting, we might forget to be appropriately skeptical of inspiring claims (“your students can catch up if you only do THIS THING!”).
Now is the time to be more skeptical, not less skeptical, of dramatic claims.
The Good News
Despite all this gloomy news, I do think we have a very sensible and realistic option right in front of us.
I propose three steps for the beginning of the next school year.
Step 1: determine what our students already know.
In previous years, I could reasonably predict that my students know this much grammar and this much about Shakespeare and this much about analyzing literature.
Well, they just don’t anymore. I need to start next year by finding out what they really do know. (Hint: it will almost certainly be less — maybe dramatically less — than they did in the past.)
Step 2: plan a realistic curriculum building from that foundation.
If we meet our students where they are, they are much likelier to learn the new ideas and procedures we teach them.
In fact, they’re also likelier to strengthen and consolidate the foundation on which they’re building.
Yes, I might feel like my students are “behind.” But they’re behind an abstract standard.
As long as they’re making good progress in learning new ideas, facts, and procedures, they’re doing exactly the right cognitive work. They won’t catch up this year.
But if they make steady progress for several years, they’ll be well back on track.
Step 3: draw on the lessons of cognitive science.
In the paragraphs above, I’ve been highly skeptical of uplifting, simplistic quick-fix claims. (“If we revolutionize education with X, our students will learn calculus in 6th grade!”)
At the same time, I do think that teachers can make steady and coherent improvements in our work. When we understand the mental processes that lead to long-term memory formation, we can teach more effectively.
We should study…
… working memory function: the core mental bottleneck that both allows and impedes learning;
… the importance of desirable difficulties — spacing, interleaving, retrieval practice — in forming long-term memories;
… the sub-components of attention that add up to concentration and understanding;
… a realistic framework for understanding student motivation.
And so forth.
Understanding these topics will not “revolutionize education overnight.”
However, teachers who design lessons and plan syllabi with these insights in mind can in fact help their students consolidate ideas more effectively.
In other words: don’t follow Bruce Willis through the park.
Instead, we should learn how learning takes place in the brain. When our teaching is guided by that knowledge, our students have the best long-term chance of getting back on track.
And sometimes, research adds nuance and insight to overly-broad generalizations.
Here’s the story:
Benefits of Enthusiasm
It seems too obvious to say that a teacher’s enthusiasm benefits learning. OF COURSE it would do that.
After all, what student wants a boring, unenthusiastic teacher?
But psychology is a science. We don’t just announce that our beliefs — even really obvious beliefs — are true.
Instead, we convert those beliefs into testable hypotheses. We run some experiments. We look at data.
IF the data from the experiment support the hypothesis, then we can start making (tentative) claims.
Once we start thinking scientifically about the effects of a teacher’s enthusiasm, we quickly run into difficult questions.
How, exactly, do we define “enthusiasm”?
One we’ve got a definition, how do we measure it?
What results are we looking for? Do we want enthusiasm to promote students’ attention? Their motivation? Their learning?
If we don’t have clear answers to those questions, we can’t proceed with a scientific answer. (We can, of course, have an answer based on personal experience. Those answers are important, but not the same thing as a scientific answer.)
Getting Specific
In a study published in 2020 — “Displayed enthusiasm attracts attention and improves recall” — several scholars took on those challenges directly.
They started by training teachers in behaviors that demonstrate high levels of enthusiasm (exuberant gestures, varied facial expression, excited & rapid speech, etc.) and low levels of enthusiasm (a few quiet gestures, fixed facial expression, vocal monotone).
Teachers then read two short passages to 4th and 5th grade public-school students. One passage was a story about a farmer; the other was a description of the habits and characteristics of dragonflies. (By the way: this distinction between the story and the description will turn out to be important.)
These passages together took about 3 minutes to read.
To measure the effect of high enthusiasm vs. low enthusiasm, researchers counted several variables, including…
… the number of seconds that students looked at the reader;
… the number of times that students smiled;
… and, the number of facts about the farmer story and dragonfly description that students recalled.
In other words: these researchers found ways to answer those scientific questions listed above. So far, so good.
Asking Tough Questions
At this point, we can ask some reasonable questions:
First, counting “number of seconds” seems like a basically plausible way of measuring attention. (We can quibble, and ask for other measures, and explain why that measure isn’t perfect, but it’s plausible on its face.)
However, I myself think that “counting smiles” seems unusually squishy for a research-based conclusion. Perhaps I’m being overly picky here, but “smiles” strike me as a highly amorphous unit of counting.
Second, the duration of the “enthusiasm” — all of 3 minutes — might not be a helpfully representative amount of time.
For instance: a teacher might delight students by telling jokes for a minute or two at the beginning of class. All that humor might get high ratings from students.
But: if that teacher keeps telling jokes, all that forced humor might get irritating after a while. So too, “high enthusiasm” might have one effect for 3 minutes and a very different effect after 30.
Third, the study measures how many facts students remember immediately after they heard the reading.
Of course, teachers don’t want students to remember just right away; we want them to remember for a long time. And the relationship between short-term and long-term memory gets really complicated.
Strategies that help immediate recall might not enhance long-term learning; Nick Soderstrom has the goods here.
Results?
So, what did the researchers find?
Any study that measures so many variables will produce LOTS of findings. Those findings will be difficult to summarize easily.
The study summarizes their findings in this sentence:
Our results confirm that displayed enthusiasm captures attention and that attention partially explains the positive effect of displayed enthusiasm on recall.
For the reasons listed above, I’m hesitant to accept that conclusion without several caveats. At a minimum, I wish it said “short-term recall.”
Even more important, I think this summary overlooks a crucial finding. Researchers found that “enthusiasm” enhanced short-term recall for the farmer story, but NOT for the dragonfly description.
This distinction leads to an important question: do you spend more time in your classroom telling (farmer-like) stories or providing (dragonfly-like) information and descriptions?
The answer to that question certainly varies from teacher to teacher, from grade to grade, from discipline to discipline, from culture to culture.
Even the most optimistic reading of this study suggests that high enthusiasm will help students remember the story, but not the information.
That’s an important distinction; one we should make clearly when offering advice to teachers.
The Bigger Picture
I myself have a hypothesis.
I suspect that a teacher’s consistent and genuine enthusiasm — not 3 minutes, not 3 weeks, but maybe 3 months or more — gradually creates a particular kind of classroom atmosphere.
That atmosphere — quietly, subtly, probably immeasurably — helps students appreciate the class work, the discipline, and the camaraderie/community.
For instance: a student recently described one of my colleagues this way: “Oh, Ms. So-and-So! She’s the ONLY reason I like English…” Knowing Ms. So-and-So’s enthusiasm for her subject, I can certainly understand why she would inspire a doubting high-school student.
And I suspect her enthusiasm ultimately means that this student learns more English.
As you can see, my hypothesis doesn’t stem from research. Heck: it’s so nebulous that I don’t think it could be researched.
In other words: do I think that a teacher’s enthusiasm ultimately enhances learning? I do. And: my belief springs not from research, but from experience and common sense. *
Moe, A., Frenzel, A. C., Au, L., & Taxer, J. L. (2021). Displayed enthusiasm attracts attention and improves recall. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 911-927.
* To be clear: I haven’t done a comprehensive search for research on teacher enthusiasm. I did plug this study into ConnectedPapers.com, and quickly scanned the results. As far as I could tell from this very brief look, research in this field is pursuing lots of helpful and optimistic leads, but doesn’t yet have confident conclusions. If you know of persuasive research looking at this topic, I hope you’ll let me know!
An amazing discovery becomes an inspiring analogy:
Researchers at BioSphere 2 noticed a bizarre series of events: their trees kept collapsing under their own weight.
Why on earth would trees collapse? It doesn’t happen outside the BioSphere; so why would it happen inside?
And then the researchers figured it out. The BioSphere doesn’t have wind.
Trees react to the stress of wind by growing stronger. If they don’t get that beneficial stress, they can’t stand up when they become adult trees.
And here’s the heart-warming bit: that’s true for humans too.
As we grow and develop, we need some modest, reasonable stresses in our lives. Those small stressors make our emotional “tree trunks” strong, so we can manage the greater stresses of adult life.
I really want to make an uplifting poster right now — don’t you?
First Things First
This story that I’ve told begins with science: “Researchers at the Biosphere…”
And so, when I read that story, I felt a small shudder of delight. I can use this story to explain to students — and parents, and teachers — the benefits of reasonable/modest stresses in their lives.
After all, it’s a GREAT story, and a great analogy.
Even better, I can share the research behind it. (That’s what I do for a living: share research with teachers, students, and parents.)
However, the website where I first read that story doesn’t link to any research.
Hmmm.
So, I started looking.
This trees-need-wind story (and its uplifting analogy) shows up frequently on the interwebs. In fact, I think I notice two waves — one around 2013, another around 2020.
But, exactly none of the articles included any scientific links — much less links supporting the claim.
Glimmers of Hope?
When I switched from Google to Google Scholar, I did find this brief report.
It appears in Science magazine — a highly reputable source — and includes this sentence:
The trunks and branches of large trees became brittle and prone to catastrophic and dangerous collapse.
So, have I found the scientific backing that this analogy was missing?
Alas, this sentence is but one part of a long catalogue of problems in BioSphere 2, as noted in that report:
Vines grew “exceptionally aggressive[ly].”
19 of 25 vertebrate species went extinct.
“All pollinators went extinct.”
CO2 levels, oxygen levels, temperature, and light exposure all went haywire.
And, NONE of these problems has much of anything to do with wind.
In fact, the word “wind” doesn’t appear in this brief article.
Simply put: as far as I can tell, the whole “wind makes trees stronger” story sounds great, but has no research backing — certainly not at Biosphere 2.
Some Conclusions
First: does wind help strengthen trees?
Maybe.
I’ve been reading about something called — believe it or not — “reaction wood.” You can read about it here.
Second: does manageable stress benefit people in the long run.
Sure.
Check out “Yerkes-Dodson.”
Third: should we use uplifting-but-false analogies to communicate important scientific truths?
As long as Learning and the Brain is here, heck no.
Here’s a practical question: should our students take notes by hand, or on laptops?
If we were confident that one strategy or the other produced more learning – factual learning, conceptual learning, ENDURING learning – then we could give our students straightforwardly useful advice.
Sadly, the research in this field has – in my opinion – produced unhelpful advice because it rests on an obviously flawed assumption.
Happily, Dr. Paul Penn (Twitter handle @Dr_Paul_Penn) recently pointed me to a study with several pertinent benefits.
First, the researchers worked with 10-year-olds, not with adults. Research with college students can be useful, but it might not always help K-12 teachers.
Second, the research took place in the students’ regular classroom, not in a psychology lab. This more realistic setting gives us greater confidence in the research’s applicability.
Third, students took notes in both a science class and in a history class. The disciplinary breadth makes its guidance more useful.
Finally, this study – for reasons that I’ll explain – makes the “obviously flawed assumption” go away.
Then I’ll explain the initial study (with the “obvious flaw”).
Then I’ll explain how the new study – by accident – makes that flaw go away.
I’ll wrap up with the big picture.
The Black Death, and Beyond
Researchers Simon Horbury and Caroline Edmonds had ten-year-olds watch videos in their history and science classes.
The history videos focused on the Black Death. The science video explored cells.
Students took laptop notes in one class, and handwritten notes in the other.
Immediately after the videos, and then again a week later, students took a multiple choice quiz. Questions covered both factual recall (“Where did the Black Death originate?”) and conceptual understanding (“Why were the wealthy less likely to be afflicted by the plague?”).
To be thorough, researchers even counted the number of words students wrote in their notes. (Believe it or not, this detail will turn out to be important at the end of this post.)
So, did it matter how students took notes?
Yup.
The study measures several variables, but the headline is: in both science and history, taking notes by hand improved learning – especially a week later.
The study includes lots of specifics — conceptual vs. factual, immediate test vs. week-later test — but that summary gets the job done.
Yes, this is a very small study (26 people at its biggest), so we shouldn’t think it’s the final word on the matter. But it offers good reason to believe that handwritten notes help.
Back to the Beginning
Like all research in this field, Horbury & Edmonds’s work rests atop a well-known study by Mueller and Oppenheimer, cleverly entitled “The Pen Is Mightier than the Laptop.”
Mueller and Oppenheimer had one group of college students take notes by hand, and another group take notes on a laptop. They found that two variables mattered for learning:
Variable #1: the number of words students wrote. Crudely put: more words in notes resulted in more learning.
This finding isn’t terribly surprising. More writing suggests more thinking; more thinking suggests more learning.
Variable #2: the degree to which students reworded the lecture. Student who put the lecture’s ideas into their own words learned more than those who simply took notes verbatim.
Again, this finding makes sense. If I simply copy down the lecturer’s ideas, I’m not thinking much. If I put them in my own words, well, now I’m thinking more.
So far, so good. No obvious flaws.
Now the study gets tricky.
The students who took handwritten notes wrote FEWER words (that’s bad), so they had to REWORD the lecture (that’s good).
The students who took laptop notes could write MORE words (that’s good), so they ended up copying the lecture VERBATIM (that’s bad).
Which pairing of good+bad is better?
In Mueller and Oppenheimer’s conclusion, handwritten notes resulted in more learning.
It’s okay to write fewer words, as long as you’re rewording as you go. Remember: more rewording = more thinking.
Obvious Flaw
I promised several paragraphs ago to point out the obvious flaw in the study. Here goes:
Mueller and Oppenheimer saw an obvious possibility: if we TRAIN laptop note takers to reword, then they’ll get BOTH benefits.
That is, students who take laptop notes correctly get the advantages of more words and more rewording.
So much thinking! So much learning!
So, the researchers ran the study again. This time they included a third group: laptop note takers who got instructions not to reword.
What happened?
Nothing. Even though they got those instructions, laptop note takers continued to copy verbatim. They still remembered less than their handwriting peers.
The Mueller and Oppenheimer study draws this conclusion: since students can’t be trained to take laptop notes correctly – and they tried! – then handwritten notes are best.
WAIT JUST A SECOND. [Please mentally insert the sound of a record scratch here.]
The researchers told students – ONCE – to change a long-held habit (verbatim copying of notes). When students failed to do so, they concluded that students can’t ever change.
In my own experience, telling my students to do something once practically NEVER has much of an effect.
Students need practice. LOTS of practice. Practice and FEEDBACK. Lots of feedback.
Obviously.
In other words, I think the Mueller and Oppenheimer study contains a conspicuous failure in logic. We shouldn’t conclude that handwritten notes are better. We SHOULD conclude that we should teach students to take laptop notes and reword as they do so.
If they can learn to do so (of course they can!), then laptop notes will be better — because they allow more words AND rewording.
Muller and Oppenheimer’s own data make that the most plausible conclusion.
Conflicting Messages
To review:
The Horbury & Edmonds study suggests that handwritten notes are better.
The Mueller and Oppenheimer study suggests (to me, at least) that laptop notes will be better – as long as students are correctly trained to reword notes as they go.
Which advice should we follow?
My answer comes back to that obscure detail I noted in parentheses.
Horbury and Edmonds, you may remember, counted the number of words students wrote. Unlike the college students, who can type faster than they write, 10-year-olds don’t.
They wrote basically the same number of words by hand as they did on the laptop.
Here’s the key point: as long as students write as fast as they type, the hypothetical advantage that I predict for college laptop note-takers simply won’t apply to younger students.
After all, laptop notes provide additional benefit only if students write more words. These younger typists don’t write more words.
Since handwritten notes produce more learning, let’s go with those!
Final Thoughts
In this post, I’ve considered two studies about note taking and laptops.
In truth, several studies explore this field. And, unsurprisingly, the results are a bit of a hodge-podge.
If you want a broader review of research in this field, check out this video from Dr. Paul Penn, who first pointed me to the Horbury and Edmonds study:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=TXLHxf__poE
Given the research we have, I DON’T think we can make emphatic, confident claims.
But, based on this study with 10-year-olds, I’m much more open to the possibility that handwritten notes are — at least in younger grades — the way to go.
Horbury, S. R., & Edmonds, C. J. (2021). Taking class notes by hand compared to typing: Effects on children’s recall and understanding. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 35(1), 55-67.
Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard: Advantages of longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological science, 25(6), 1159-1168.
More precisely: if I want to believe a research finding, I become very suspicious of it. After all: it’s easy to fool me when I want to be fooled.
Specifically: I’m an outdoors guy. I’ve worked at summer camps for ages, and love a good walk in the forests around Walden Pond.
So, when I read research showing that even a brief nature walk produces cognitive benefits, I’m both VERY EXCITED and EXTRA SKEPTICAL.
Let’s start with the assumption that it’s just not true.
Persuade Me
The research I’m speaking of is in fact a review article; it summarizes and compares the results of 14 studies. (The review article was flagged by Professor Dan Willingham, one of the leaders in translating science research for the classroom.)
These 14 studies shared important commonalities:
First: they looked at “one-time” exposure to nature. They didn’t look at — say — outdoor education programs. Instead, they looked at — say — a brisk walk in a park near the school.
Second: these “one-time exposures” were all relatively brief — somewhere between 10 and 90 minutes.
Third: these “brief, one-time exposures” did NOT deliberately focus the participants on nature. That is: students didn’t walk in the park to learn about trees and birds. They walked in the park to have the experience of walking in the park.
I might be skeptical about one study. I might be skeptical of two studies. But if 14 studies (or a substantial percentage of them) all reach the same conclusion … well, maybe I’ll be persuaded.
Equally interesting: these studies ran the K-16 gamut. We’re not looking at a narrow age-range here: more like two decades.
Conclusions (and Questions)
So, what did this potentially-persuasive bunch of studies show?
YES: in 12 of the 14 studies, brief, one-time, passive exposure to nature does benefit cognition.
More specifically, researchers found benefits in measures of directed attention and working memory.
They looked for, but did not find, benefits in measures of inhibition (another important executive function).
And, crucially, they did not measure academic performance. If a walk in nature enhances attention and working memory, we can reasonably predict that it will also improve learning. But: these studies did not measure that prediction.
Because this review covers so many studies, it’s easy to get lost in the details.
One point I do want to emphasize: the impressive variety of “exposures.”
Some students walked or played in a park, woods, or nature trail.
Some simply sat and read outdoors.
Amazingly, some walked on a treadmill watching a simulated nature trail on the monitor.
In fact, some simply sat in a classroom “with windows open on to green space.”
In other words: it doesn’t take much nature to get the benefits of nature.
Inevitable Caveats
First: in these studies, exposure to nature helped restore attention and working memory capacity that had been strained.
It did not somehow increase overall attention and WM capacity in an enduring way. Students recovered faster. But they didn’t end up with more of these capacities than they started with.
Second: most of these “exposures” included some modest physical activity.
How much (if any) of the benefit came from that physical exertion, instead of the greenery?
We don’t yet know.
A Skeptic Converted?
I have to say, I’m strongly swayed by this review.
In the past, I’ve seen studies that might contradict this set of conclusions.
But the number of studies, the variety of conditions, the variety of cognitive measures, and the range of ages all seem very encouraging.
Perhaps we can’t (yet) say that “research tells us” brief exposures to nature benefit students. But I feel much more comfortable speculating that this belief just might be true.