2025 – Page 4 – Education & Teacher Conferences Skip to main content
Nerd Alert: Focusing on Definitions
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

You come to Learning and the Brain conferences — and to this blog — because you want research-based insight into teaching and learning.

We sincerely hope that you get lots of those insights, and feel inspired by them.

At the same time, all sorts of work has to go on behind the scenes to make sure such advice has merit. Much of that work seems tedious, but all of it is important.

For instance: definitions.

When researchers explore a particular topic — say, “learning” — they have to measure something — say, “how much someone learned.”

To undertake that measurement, they rely on a definition of the thing to be measured — for example: “getting correct answers on a subsequent test = learning.”

A close-up photograh of a dictionary lying open.

Of course, skeptics might reject that definition: “tests don’t reveal learning. Only real world application reveals learning.”

Because these skeptics have a different definition, they need to measure in a different way. And, of course, they might come to a different conclusion about the value of the teaching practice being measured.

In other words:

If I define learning as “getting answers right on a test,” I might conclude that the Great Watson Teaching Method works.

If you define learning as “using new concepts spontaneously in the real world,” you might conclude that the Great Watson Teaching method is a bust.

The DEFINITION tells researchers what to MEASURE; it thereby guides our ultimate CONCLUSIONS.

A Case in Point

I recently read an article, by Hambrick, Macnamara, and Oswald, about deliberate practice.

Now, if you’ve spent time at a Learning and the Brain conference in the last decade, you’ve heard researcher K. Anders Ericsson and others present on this topic. It means, basically, “practicing with the specific intention of getting better.”

According to Ericsson and others, deliberate practice is THE key to developing expertise in almost any field: sports, music, chess, academics, professional life.

Notice, however, that I included the slippery word ‘basically’ in my definition two sentences ago. I wrote: “it means, basically, ‘practicing with the specific intention of getting better.’ ”

That “basically” means I’m giving a rough definition, not precise one.

But, for the reasons explained above, we shouldn’t use research to give advice without precise definitions.

As Hambrick, Macnamara, and Oswald detail, deliberate practice has a frustratingly flexible definition. For instance:

  • Can students create their own deliberate practice regimens? Or do they need professionals/teachers to create them and give feedback?
  • Does group/team practice count, or must deliberate practice be individual?

As the authors detail, the answers to those questions change over time.

Even more alarmingly, they seem to change depending on the context. In some cases, Ericsson and his research partners hold up studies as examples of deliberate practice, but say that Hambrick’s team should not include them in meta-analyses evaluating the effectiveness of deliberate practice.

(The back-n-forth here gets very technical.)

Although the specifics of this debate quickly turn mind-numbing, the debate itself points to a troubling conclusion: because we can’t define deliberate practice with much confidence, we should hesitate to make strong research claims about the benefits of deliberate practice.

Because — again — research depends on precise definitions.

Curiouser and Curiouser

The argument above reminded me of another study that I read several years ago. Because that study uses lots of niche-y technical language, I’m going to simplify it a fair bit. But its headlines were clear:

Project-based learning helps students learn; direct instruction does not.

Because the “constructivist” vs. “direct instruction” debate rages so passionately, I was intrigued to find a study making such a strong claim.

One of my first questions will sound familiar: “how, precisely, did the researchers define ‘project-based learning’ and ‘direct instruction.’ ”

This study started with these definitions:

Direct instruction: “lecturing with passive listening.”

Constructivism: “problem-solving opportunities … that provide meaning. Students learn by collaboratively solving authentic, real-life problems, developing explanations and communicating ideas.”

To confirm their hypothesis, the reseachers had one group of biology students (the “constructivism” group) do an experiment where they soaked chicken bones in vinegar to see how flexible the bones became.

The “direct instruction” students copied the names of 206 bones from the chalkboard into their notebooks.

After even this brief description, you might have some strong reactions to this study.

First: OF COURSE students don’t learn much from copying the names of 206 bones. Who seriously thinks that they do? No cognitive scientist I’ve ever met.

Second: no one — and I mean NO ONE — who champions direct instruction would accept the definition as “lecturing with passive listening.”

In other words: we might be excited (or alarmed) to discover research championing PBL over direct instruction. But we shouldn’t use this reseach to make decisions about that choice because it relies on obviously inaccurate definitions.

(If you’re interested in this example — or this study — I’ve written about it extensively in my book, The Goldilocks Map.)

In Brief:

It might seem nerdy to focus so stubbornly on research definitions. If we’re serious about following research-informed guidance for our teaching, we really must.


Hambrick, D. Z., Macnamara, B. N., & Oswald, F. L. (2020). Is the deliberate practice view defensible? A review of evidence and discussion of issues. Frontiers in Psychology11, 1134.

Finding a Framework for Trauma
Andrew Watson
Andrew Watson

Although education itself encourages detailed and nuanced understandings of complex ideas, the field of education often rushes to extremes.

According to the loudest voices:

  • Artificial intelligence will either transform education for the better, or make us all dumber.
  • Memorization is either an essential foundation for all learning, or “drill and kill.”
  • A growth mindset will either motivate students to new successes, or delude teachers into this out-dated fad (“yet” schmet).

And so forth.

This tendency to extremes seems especially powerful at the intersection of education and trauma.

Depending on your source and your decade, trauma is

  • Either a problem so rare that it doesn’t merit discussion, or
  • a problem so pervasive and debilitating that we need to redesign education.

How can we find a steady, helpful, realistic path without rushing to extremes?

A Useful Start

If we’re going to think about trauma, we should start with a definition of it.

A thousand-word blog post can’t get into the subtleties, but here’s a useful starting place:

“Trauma is a response to an event or series of events that overwhelms an individual’s capacity to cope.”

In that sentence, “overwhelmed” means a serious and ongoing response — not short-term unhappiness (even if intense).

Symptoms of being “overwhelmed” might include dissociation, flashbacks, night terrors, drug addiction, or major depression.

Note: unlike trauma, stress puts pressure on — but does not inherently overwhelm — coping capacity.

Thoughtful people might not agree with the sentences above, but I think most people will agree that they’re an honest attempt to describe a complex mental state.

The First Pendulum

Discussions of trauma — especially the extreme versions — begin with its sources.

When I started teaching, in the 1980s, our school — quite literally — NEVER discussed trauma. (To be fair, I should say: “I don’t remember ever discussing trauma.”)

A closeup of a man sitting with his forearms resting on his legs; his hands are tensely knotted.

The implied message: “trauma probably happens somewhere to some people. But it’s so rare, and so unlikely to be a part of our students’ lives, we’re not going to use precious faculty time to focus on it.”

In brief: “the causes of trauma aren’t relevant to teachers.”

Since those days, our profession has rightly recognized that trauma DOES happen. It does happen to our students and in their families and communities. The causes of trauma are absolutely relevant to teachers.

And yet, because our profession tends to extremes, I now hear the flipside of that earlier casual dismissal. Instead of being rare and almost irrelevant, trauma is common and pervasive.

One sign of this trend: a lengthening list of common occurances that cause trauma. Perfectly typical stressors — being cut from a sports team, getting a bad grade — are reframed as traumatic.

I’ve even seen the claim that “things that we don’t get to experience can be traumatic.” While missed chances can be disappointing, even stressful, it’s just hard to see how they fit the definition of trauma.

The list of symptoms has also grown. E.g.: “procrastination is a sign of trauma.”

Now, I don’t doubt that some people who have experienced trauma procrastinate; I also don’t doubt that almost everyone procrastinates. Traumatized people might procrastinate, but not all people who procrastinate have experienced trauma.

To avoid being caught up in this race to the extremes, I think it helps to keep the definition in mind: a response to an event or series of events that overwhelms an individual’s capacity to cope.

Such events do happen to our students — but not frequently, and not to all of them.

The Second Pendulum

While we negotiate this first pendulum (“trauma doesn’t happen/is universal”), we also watch a second one swing back and forth.

Old school: “least said, soonest mended. On those infrequent occasions when trauma really happens, we should all just keep going and not make a big deal about it.”

Pendulum swing: “a traumatized student is literally incapable of paying attention or learning. Schooling as we know it should come to a halt.”

This second statement is usually accompanied by neuroscience terminology, starting with “amygdala.”

I was reminded of this pendulum swing at the most recent Learning and the Brain conference in Boston — specifically in a keynote address by George A. Bonanno.

Dr. Bonanno has been studying trauma for decades; in his talk, he focused on the symptoms that follow trauma.

He and his team have been running studies and aggregating data, and he showed graphs representing conclusions based on more than 60 trajectory analyses.

To present his complex findings as simply as possible:

  • Roughly 10% of people who experience trauma have enduring symptoms;
  • Less than 10% start without symptoms, but symptoms develop over time and persist;
  • Roughly 20% initially experience symptoms, but recover over two years;
  • The rest never repond with serious symptoms.

In other words: in Bonanno’s research, two years after trauma, roughly 80% of people do not experience troubling symptoms.

For this reason, by the way, Bonanno does not speak of “traumatic events” but of “potentially traumatic events.”

That is: an event has the potential to create trauma symptoms in a person. But something like two-thirds of people do not experience trauma in response to that potentially traumatic event. (And another 10% recover from those symptoms in a year or two.)

Towards a Balanced Framework

How then should teachers think about trauma in schools.

First: we can avoid the extremes.

Yes, trauma does happen.

No, it isn’t common. (Bad grades aren’t traumatic.)

Yes, schools and teachers should respond appropriately to the trauma that students experience.

No, not everyone responds to trauma the same way. Most people react to potentially traumatic events without trauma symptoms (or recover over time).

Second: within this nuanced perspective, we should acknowledge the importance of responding to trauma appropriately.

That is: events that potentially create trauma might be rare; most people might not respond to them with trauma symptoms.

And: our students who do experience trauma symtoms deserve informed and sympathetic response.

By way of analogy: something like 3% of K-12 students are on the autism spectrum. That’s a relatively small number. And: those students deserve the best education we can provide.

If 3% of our students experience trauma symptoms (I have no idea what the actual percentage is), they too deserve our professional best.

Attempting a Summary

In our profession, we have all too frequently overlooked and downplayed the trauma that some of our students experience. As we try to correct that serious error, we should not commit another error by seeing trauma everywhere, and by assuming it debilitates everyone.


 

A Final Note:

To keep this post a readable length, I have not discussed ACES scores. Depending on the response this post gets, I may return to that topic in a future post.